全文获取类型
收费全文 | 256篇 |
免费 | 0篇 |
专业分类
256篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 8篇 |
2023年 | 6篇 |
2022年 | 11篇 |
2021年 | 10篇 |
2020年 | 15篇 |
2019年 | 4篇 |
2018年 | 15篇 |
2017年 | 12篇 |
2016年 | 11篇 |
2015年 | 12篇 |
2014年 | 12篇 |
2013年 | 48篇 |
2012年 | 12篇 |
2011年 | 7篇 |
2010年 | 17篇 |
2009年 | 14篇 |
2008年 | 8篇 |
2007年 | 4篇 |
2006年 | 2篇 |
2005年 | 2篇 |
2004年 | 1篇 |
2002年 | 1篇 |
2001年 | 2篇 |
1999年 | 2篇 |
1998年 | 2篇 |
1997年 | 1篇 |
1996年 | 2篇 |
1992年 | 2篇 |
1990年 | 1篇 |
1985年 | 1篇 |
1984年 | 1篇 |
1982年 | 2篇 |
1981年 | 4篇 |
1978年 | 1篇 |
1977年 | 1篇 |
1973年 | 1篇 |
1972年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有256条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
241.
The aim of this study was to evaluate the relationship between three perceived parental attitudes and shyness, testing self-esteem
and fear of negative evaluation as mediators. The study used a total of 492 undergraduate students in Turkey. Data was collected
through measures of shyness, fear of negative evaluation, self-esteem, and perceived parental attitudes. The proposed model
was estimated using path analysis. Goodness of fit statistics showed that the model fit the data well. Results indicated that
parental acceptance/involvement predicted self-esteem; perceived parental strictness/supervision predicted fear of negative
evaluation; and perceived parental psychological autonomy predicted self-esteem. Self-esteem partially mediated the relationship
between parental acceptance/involvement and shyness, whereas it fully mediated the relationship between parental psychological
autonomy and shyness. Fear of negative evaluation fully mediated the relationship between parental strictness/supervision
and shyness. In addition, the relationship between self-esteem and shyness was partially mediated by fear of negative evaluation.
The findings provided evidence for the propositions that parental attitudes are important in the development of self-image
and fears of rejection which result in the experience of shyness. 相似文献
242.
Sverker SÖrlin 《Studies in Philosophy and Education》2002,21(4-5):377-388
The discussion of universities anddemocracy has conventionally dealt first andforemost with the curriculum, or with thespirit of openness and tolerance whichcharacterises the scientific inquiry. In thisarticle I have added a discussion of thesituatedness of knowledge and knowledgeproduction, and, consequently, a discussion ofthe situated character of other roles of theuniversity, including the democratic role. Inthe light of the regress of political partiesand traditional popular movements – phenomenawhich seem to be true both as regardsmembership numbers and as regards level ofactivity – the role as a locally andregionally situated meeting place for avitalisation and defence of democracy seems apromising one for the contemporary university.The article has also emphasised the linksbetween culture, social capital (``the socialfabric') in the surrounding society, and theeconomic growth and welfare of the city andregion. With concepts such as Bildung (oreducation), civic competence (or civility), andculture, and I have tried to draw our attentionto the actual ``places of knowledge,' whoseimportance will certainly grow in the years tocome. The care, the cultivation, and thequalification of these places into supportinginfrastructures for cultural life, creativity,industry, and democracy should be seen as acoherent, holistic mission. It needsrecognition, and articulation, by all those whoare interested in the university as asignificant social factor: politicians,industry, the cultural sector, localcommunities, and, obviously, the academiccommunity itself. 相似文献
243.
Purpose The aim of this study was to systematize findings in role stress research into original and conceptually important abstract
higher-order constructs and to develop and test a comprehensive structural equation model that examined such expanded conceptualizations
of antecedents and consequences to entrepreneur role stress.
Design/Methodology/Approach Model tests were performed on data from a sample of 282 Swedish entrepreneurs (a usable rate of 22.5%) engaged in their first-year
of venture activities. We used structural equation modeling, mediation tests and tests for common method bias to test the
appropriateness of the model.
Findings We found that role stress can be explained by expansions of lower-ranked, less abstract constructs embedded in a multiple-indicator
model of venture technology, venture environment, and entrepreneur personality. The analysis confirmed that role stress is
an important mediator and that it has pronounced relationships to expanded conceptualizations of role-related rewards and
exhaustion.
Implications This study advance role stress theory and existing knowledge about entrepreneurs and entrepreneurship when it introduces role
stress and suggests it mediates the effects from personality traits, organizational and environmental characteristics, on
higher-order conceptualizations of rewards and exhaustion.
Originality/Value With contributions from several distinct disciplines over a half decade, prior research has not paid much attention to show
how role stress research can benefit from more abstract conceptualizations and empirical evaluation. By synthesizing and developing
expanded higher-level conceptualizations that link diversities, we show that expanded conceptualizations effectively enable
to introduce role stress to entrepreneurs and entrepreneurship. 相似文献
244.
Özlem Şimşekoğlu Timo Lajunen 《Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour》2009,12(3):235-241
Seat belts are effective safety devices for protecting car occupants from injuries and fatalities in road vehicle accidents. Seat belt use has been reported to be related to some health and driving-related behaviors. The aim of the present study was to investigate to what degree seat belt use can be seen as health behavior or driver behavior. Participants were 252 licensed Turkish drivers (180 males, 72 females) with the mean age of 30.8 (SD = 12.15). A questionnaire including questions related to health-related behaviors, driver behaviors and seat belt use was used. Results of factor analysis showed that seat belt use in front seat grouped with driver behaviors (e.g., driving errors and violations) but not with health-related behaviors (e.g., healthy diet and sports participation). Regression analyses showed that seat belt use in back seat; and, regular walking and adequate sleep were positively related to seat belt use in front seat, whereas being male, driving errors and smoking frequency were negatively related to seat belt use in front seat. The present findings suggest that seat belt use can be considered in the context of driver behaviors such as driving errors and violations. 相似文献
245.
246.
247.
Özlem Ece Demir‐Lira Salomi S. Asaridou Anjali Raja Beharelle Anna E. Holt Susan Goldin‐Meadow Steven L. Small 《Developmental science》2018,21(5)
Gesture is an integral part of children's communicative repertoire. However, little is known about the neurobiology of speech and gesture integration in the developing brain. We investigated how 8‐ to 10‐year‐old children processed gesture that was essential to understanding a set of narratives. We asked whether the functional neuroanatomy of gesture–speech integration varies as a function of (1) the content of speech, and/or (2) individual differences in how gesture is processed. When gestures provided missing information not present in the speech (i.e., disambiguating gesture; e.g., “pet” + flapping palms = bird), the presence of gesture led to increased activity in inferior frontal gyri, the right middle temporal gyrus, and the left superior temporal gyrus, compared to when gesture provided redundant information (i.e., reinforcing gesture; e.g., “bird” + flapping palms = bird). This pattern of activation was found only in children who were able to successfully integrate gesture and speech behaviorally, as indicated by their performance on post‐test story comprehension questions. Children who did not glean meaning from gesture did not show differential activation across the two conditions. Our results suggest that the brain activation pattern for gesture–speech integration in children overlaps with—but is broader than—the pattern in adults performing the same task. Overall, our results provide a possible neurobiological mechanism that could underlie children's increasing ability to integrate gesture and speech over childhood, and account for individual differences in that integration. 相似文献
248.
The present research investigated positive and negative behavioural intentions towards Syrian refugees in Turkey. The behavioural intentions were examined in relation to national identification, perception of threat, and humanitarian concerns. A questionnaire was conducted among Turkish participants (n = 605) and the results showed that respondents made a distinction between negative and positive behavioural intentions towards Syrian refugees. Further, higher national identification was associated with more negative and less positive behavioural intentions, and perception of threat was responsible for these associations. In addition, humanitarian concern was associated with more positive behavioural intentions and less negative ones. Additionally, stronger humanitarian concern weakened the association between threat perceptions and negative behavioural intentions but also strengthened the association between higher threat and lower positive behavioural intentions. 相似文献
249.
Jenny Meyer Caisa Öster Mia Ramklint Johan Isaksson 《Scandinavian journal of psychology》2020,61(5):671-678
Little is known about how adolescents with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) experience participating in group therapy, an important factor to consider when developing treatment methods for this age group. This study aimed to explore how adolescents with ADHD experience participating in a structured skills training group program based on dialectical behavioral therapy. Semi-structured interviews were conducted with 20 adolescents (15–18 years of age) with ADHD after participating in a structured skills training group. The interviews were transcribed verbatim and qualitative content analysis were used to analyze the text. The participants emphasized the value of meeting other adolescents with ADHD and the opportunity to exchange experiences, strategies and tips. Participating in the group treatment made the adolescents realize that they were not alone, and feelings of togetherness and an increased acceptance of themselves were described. The participants associated the treatment with elevated knowledge and understanding, for example, about ADHD, their own functioning and helpful strategies. They also described emotional and behavioral changes, such as higher self-esteem, fewer inter-personal conflicts and improved concentration. Activating and experiential exercises were considered important elements of the treatment, and the participants expressed a need for a variation of exercises, as well as more time for practicing skills, discussions and breaks. The results indicate that the group format add an extra value to the treatment and that the use of an active approach throughout the treatment is of importance for this group of patients. 相似文献
250.
We examined recollective experience in odor memory as a function of age, intention to learn, and familiarity. Young and older adults studied a set of familiar and unfamiliar odors with incidental or intentional encoding instructions. At recognition, participants indicated whether their response was based on explicit recollection (remembering), a feeling of familiarity (knowing), or guessing. The results indicated no age-related differences in the distribution of experiential responses for unfamiliar odors. By contrast, for familiar odors the young demonstrated more explicit recollection than the older adults, who produced more know and guess responses. Intention to learn was unrelated to recollective experience. In addition, the observed age differences in remember responses for familiar odors were eliminated when odor naming was statistically controlled. This suggests that age-related deficits in activating specific odor knowledge (i.e., odor names) play an important role for age differences in recollective experience of olfactory information. 相似文献