全文获取类型
收费全文 | 3547篇 |
免费 | 59篇 |
专业分类
3606篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 51篇 |
2023年 | 50篇 |
2022年 | 53篇 |
2021年 | 68篇 |
2020年 | 98篇 |
2019年 | 63篇 |
2018年 | 139篇 |
2017年 | 147篇 |
2016年 | 135篇 |
2015年 | 119篇 |
2014年 | 110篇 |
2013年 | 390篇 |
2012年 | 217篇 |
2011年 | 163篇 |
2010年 | 119篇 |
2009年 | 118篇 |
2008年 | 146篇 |
2007年 | 153篇 |
2006年 | 91篇 |
2005年 | 109篇 |
2004年 | 82篇 |
2003年 | 67篇 |
2002年 | 67篇 |
2001年 | 71篇 |
2000年 | 69篇 |
1999年 | 48篇 |
1998年 | 23篇 |
1997年 | 23篇 |
1996年 | 18篇 |
1995年 | 16篇 |
1994年 | 16篇 |
1992年 | 21篇 |
1991年 | 16篇 |
1990年 | 35篇 |
1989年 | 27篇 |
1988年 | 17篇 |
1987年 | 28篇 |
1986年 | 22篇 |
1985年 | 20篇 |
1984年 | 23篇 |
1983年 | 17篇 |
1981年 | 16篇 |
1979年 | 28篇 |
1975年 | 23篇 |
1974年 | 25篇 |
1973年 | 19篇 |
1972年 | 15篇 |
1971年 | 16篇 |
1970年 | 19篇 |
1968年 | 18篇 |
排序方式: 共有3606条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
851.
Ayse K. Uskul Susan E. Cross Cansu Alözkan Berna Gerçek-Swing Bilge Ataca Ceren Günsoy 《Cognition & emotion》2013,27(6):1057-1075
The main goal of the current research is to investigate emotional reactions to situations that implicate honour in Turkish and northern American cultural groups. In Studies 1A and 1B, participants rated the degree to which a variety of events fit their prototypes for honour-related situations. Both Turkish and American participants evaluated situations generated by their co-nationals as most central to their prototypes of honour-related situations. Study 2 examined emotional responses to Turkish or US-generated situations that varied in centrality to the prototype. Highly central situations and Turkish-generated situations elicited stronger emotions than less central situations and US-generated situations. Americans reported higher levels of positive emotions in response to honour-enhancing situations than did Turkish participants. These findings demonstrate that the prototypes of honour relevant situations differ for Turkish and northern American people, and that Turkish honour relevant situations are more emotion-laden than are northern American honour relevant situations. 相似文献
852.
Peter Fischer Julia K. Fischer Nilüfer Aydin Dieter Frey 《Basic and applied social psychology》2013,35(4):340-347
The present research investigated the impact of a social information source's physical attractiveness on selective exposure to decision-relevant information. In two studies, support was found for the hypothesis that physically attractive information sources lead decision makers into being more selective in both searching for (Studies 1 and 2; selective exposure) and evaluating (Study 2; biased assimilation) decision-relevant information. In both studies, the effect was driven by differences in the selection and evaluation of decision-consistent information, that is, physically attractive information sources led to increases in the perceived quality of decision-consistent information as well as increased selective exposure to this information. Theoretical and practical implications are discussed. 相似文献
853.
Tobias Greitemeyer Dirk O. Mügge Irina Bollermann 《Basic and applied social psychology》2013,35(3):252-258
The use of social networking sites, such as Facebook, provides ample opportunities for the pursuit of interpersonal connection but may also bring to mind one's social isolation. The present research examined the effects of interpersonal neglect (i.e., low number of responding Facebook friends) on the satisfaction of fundamental human needs. Two studies revealed that receiving few responses from one's Facebook friends threatens the needs for belonging, self-esteem, control, and meaningful existence. These effects were observable over and above the impact of general social connection to others (i.e., total number of Facebook friends) and tone of responses. 相似文献
854.
Günther Eder 《逻辑史和逻辑哲学》2013,34(1):68-78
In the last two decades, there has been increasing interest in a re-evaluation of Frege's stance towards consistency- and independence proofs. Papers by several authors deal with Frege's views on these topics. In this note, I want to discuss one particular problem, which seems to be a main reason for Frege's reluctant attitude towards his own proposed method of proving the independence of axioms, namely his view that thoughts, that is, intensional entities are the objects of metatheoretical investigations. This stands in contrast to more straightforward interpretations, which claim that Frege's hesitancy is mainly due to worries concerning the logical constants or what counts as a logical inference. 相似文献
855.
Erik Lindberg Joakim Wincent Daniel Örtqvist 《Journal of applied social psychology》2013,43(2):263-274
This study suggests that stressors can be productive for self‐efficacy and that the influence of stressors on self‐efficacy is nonlinear. Analyses were conducted with ordinary least squares regression on a dataset covering responses from 311 deans in Swedish secondary schools. Results support the hypothesized U‐shape relationship between role conflict and self‐efficacy and the inverted U‐shape relationship between role ambiguity and self‐efficacy. Thus, findings offer evidence for nonlinear effects of stressors on the level of incumbents' self‐efficacy. This research has implications for further research focused on the association between role stressors and self‐efficacy. 相似文献
856.
We have integrated the basic psychological needs approach from self‐determination theory with motive disposition theory in order to enhance the prediction of flow experience in sports. We hypothesize that an environment that enables people to fulfill their basic psychological needs for competence and social relatedness results in flow. Additionally, we assume that the effect of competence need satisfaction is moderated by the achievement motive and that the effect of need‐for‐relatedness satisfaction is moderated by the affiliation motive. Four studies show the expected positive effects of need satisfaction on flow and further confirm that high achievement and affiliation‐motivated individuals benefit more from competence and relatedness sports environments, respectively, than individuals low in these motives. 相似文献
857.
Dr. Hans-Jürgen Wirth 《International Forum of Psychoanalysis》2013,22(2):94-107
Abstract The existence of man is distinguished by its split state: man stands in the middle of life yet still has an awareness of his own death. He has to compensate whatever is missing in him naturally at the societal level, created as culture, and at the individual level through creativity. Rank investigated the human ‘creative drive’, the anthropological aspiration to express oneself in creative works, and to overcome the fear of death with its help. Freud admired poets and artists, whose achievements he could not psychoanalytically access, but he considered science superior to the harmless and naïve arts. There are two anthropological radicals: premature birth and the consciousness of death. Freud's massive fear of death made it difficult for him to acknowledge the problem of death appropriately. In Rank's concept, the development of human creativity contributes towards the fear of death being alleviated so that the knowledge of death can be integrated into life; creativity belongs to the fundamental opportunities of man that may enable him to find a way through neurosis. Failure is as much a part of life as is creativity: those who do not experience and accept life in its tragic dimension are denied creativity. Only a creative person who accepts his partial failure finds the strength to continue to be creative without his imperfect work leading to the ritual repetition of the same thing again and again, that is, getting stuck in recidivism. 相似文献
858.
Objectives: Subjective age is an important correlate of health, well-being, and longevity. So far, little is known about short-term variability in subjective age and the circumstances under which individuals feel younger/older in daily life. This study examined whether (a) older adults’ felt age fluctuates on a day-to-day basis, (b) daily changes in health, stressors, and affect explain fluctuations in felt age, and (c) the daily associations between felt age and health, stressors, or affect are time-ordered.Method: Using an eight-day daily diary approach, N = 43 adults (60–96 years, M = 74.65, SD = 8.19) filled out daily questionnaires assessing subjective age, health, daily stressors, and affect. Data were analysed using multilevel modelling.Main outcome measures: Subjective age, health, daily stressors, affect.Results: Intra-individual variability in felt age was not explained by time but by short-term variability in other variables. Specifically, on days when participants experienced more than average health problems, stress, or negative affect they felt older than on days with average health, stress, or negative affect. No time-ordered effects were found.Conclusion: Bad health, many stressors, and negative affective experiences constitute circumstances under which older adults feel older than they typically do. Thus, daily measures of subjective age could be markers of health and well-being. 相似文献
859.
Benjamin Schüz Julia K. Wolff Lisa M. Warner Jochen P. Ziegelmann Susanne Wurm 《Psychology & health》2013,28(4):442-457
Objective: Previous research on illness perceptions has focused on single illnesses, but most adults over 65 suffer from multiple illnesses (multimorbidity). This study tests three competing operationalisations of multiple illness perceptions in predicting physical functioning and adherence: (1) main effects and interactions model; (2) peak model with effects of the most prominent illness perception; and (3) combined model with averaged illness perceptions over multiple illnesses.Design: Longitudinal study in N?=?215 individuals (65–86?years) with multimorbidity at two measurement points over six months. Participants filled in two Brief Illness Perception Questionnaires (B-IPQ) on their two most severe illnesses.Main outcome measures: Physical functioning, medication adherence.Results: Factor analyses suggest that the B-IPQ contains three dimensions; consequences, control and timeline. Multiple regression models fit the data best for (1) and (3). Timeline (β?=??.18) and control (β?=?.21) predict adherence in (1); consequences (β?=?.16) and control (β?=?.20) in (3). Physical functioning was significantly predicted by interacting control beliefs in (1; β?=?.13), by peak consequences in (2; β?=??.14) and by consequences (β?=??.15) in (3).Conclusions: Individuals with multimorbidity hold both distinct and combined perceptions about their illnesses. To understand individual responses to multimorbidity, perceptions about all illnesses and multimorbidity as entity should be examined. 相似文献
860.