首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   910篇
  免费   33篇
  2021年   7篇
  2020年   14篇
  2019年   23篇
  2018年   24篇
  2017年   29篇
  2016年   27篇
  2015年   14篇
  2014年   27篇
  2013年   101篇
  2012年   28篇
  2011年   39篇
  2010年   17篇
  2009年   19篇
  2008年   33篇
  2007年   32篇
  2006年   23篇
  2005年   21篇
  2004年   28篇
  2003年   21篇
  2002年   22篇
  2001年   21篇
  2000年   20篇
  1999年   14篇
  1998年   7篇
  1997年   9篇
  1996年   8篇
  1995年   13篇
  1994年   11篇
  1992年   11篇
  1991年   8篇
  1990年   9篇
  1989年   9篇
  1988年   9篇
  1987年   13篇
  1986年   16篇
  1985年   11篇
  1984年   12篇
  1983年   9篇
  1982年   8篇
  1981年   13篇
  1979年   17篇
  1978年   7篇
  1977年   15篇
  1976年   10篇
  1975年   7篇
  1974年   11篇
  1973年   10篇
  1971年   8篇
  1970年   10篇
  1968年   7篇
排序方式: 共有943条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
171.
The method of triads for psychophysical judgments was analyzed in terms of the unidimensional unfolding model of Coombs (1964). This model predicts laterality effects on the stochastic transitivity of triples for which existing models have been unable to account. An experiment was conducted in which triadic judgments were made on circles differing in area. The results confirmed the stochastic transitivity predictions, but a quantitative formulation of the model proved unsatisfactory.  相似文献   
172.
A central assumption in portfolio theory of risk preference is that for games with expected value constant the preference ordering of an individual should exhibit a single peaked preference function. This assumption was tested by determining the level of stochastic transitivity satisfied by triples of pairwise preference proportions among eight gambles. In only a small percentage of cases were the predicted levels of stochastic transitivity not satisfied. It was concluded that the results provided strong support for portfolio theory.  相似文献   
173.
174.
The lever pressing of four food- and water-deprived rats was reinforced on concurrent variable-interval schedules. Food reinforced one response, and water reinforced the other. Response rates in baseline were higher in the food component than in the water component. After response patterns and body weights had stabilized, the animals were given access to either food only, water only, both food and water, or neither food nor water (baseline) before daily sessions. Giving food before a session decreased per cent time in the food component, decreased overall response rates for food, and increased overall response rates for water. Giving water before a session increased per cent time in the food component, increased overall response rates for food, and decreased overall response rates for water. Giving both food and water before a session resulted in a combination of prefeeding and prewatering effects. More food and more water were consumed when both were available than when only one was available before a session.  相似文献   
175.
Instruments, designed to assess values which affect the motivation to work and aspiration levels, were administered to over 600 Negro male seniors in Texas high schools. Results indicate that the degree and duration of school desegregation these students have experienced, ranging from less than 1 to more than 5 years, have had little effect on work values and occupational aspirations.  相似文献   
176.
177.
178.
Teachers and parents as researchers using multiple baseline designs   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
Two teachers and a parent used three basic multiple baseline designs to investigate the effects of systematic reinforcement and punishment procedures in the classroom and at home. (1) A fifth-grade teacher concurrently measured the same behavior (tardiness) in three stimulus situations (after morning, noon, and afternoon recesses). Posting the names of pupils on a chart titled "Today's Patriots" was made contingent on being on time after the noon recess, then successively also the morning and afternoon recesses. Tardiness was reduced to near zero rates at the points where contingencies were applied. (2) A highschool teacher recorded the same behavior (daily French-quiz grades) of three students. She then successively applied the same consequences (staying after school for individual tutoring for D and F grades) for each student. At the points where the contingency was applied, D and F grades were eliminated. (3) A mother concurrently measured three different behaviors (clarinet practice, Campfire project work, reading) of her 10-yr-old daughter. She successively applied the same contingency (going to bed early) for less than 30 min spent engaged in one after another of the behaviors. Marked increases in the behaviors were observed at the points where the contingency was applied.  相似文献   
179.
The present study examined the relations between field independence and simultaneous processing of preschool children as measured by the Preschool Embedded Figures Test (PEFT) and subtests from the Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children (K-ABC), respectively. Data partially supported the assumption that field-independent children would perform significantly better on simultaneous processing tasks in general. Significant age and sex differences on field-independence/field-dependence and simultaneous-processing abilities were found among the embedded figures task and selected portions of the K-ABC. Analysis of embedded figures further indicated age differences, but no significant sex differences.  相似文献   
180.
The conversations of 36 41/2 to 5-year-olds differing in race (black and white) and social class (professional and working class) and the adults with whom they spoke were tape-recorded during play and teaching time at preschool and dinnertime at home. Usage of cognitive words was analyzed for six levels of meaning that differed in depth of processing from reference to (1) perception and attention, (2) recognition, (3) fact recall, (4) understanding, (5) metacognition, and (6) evaluation of presuppositions. Although the rank order of usage was the same, children devoted less of their lexicon to the three higher levels of meaning than adults. Even in adults, perceptual references predominated. Use of higher-level meaning was less prominent in school and in the black working-class population. There were significant correlations between exposure to adult conversations with high-level meaning and child use of those meanings, and between the diversity of cognitive vocabulary in children and adults, but those correlations were smallest in the black working-class population.Funds to the project on which this paper is based were given by The Carnegie Corporation of New York to William S. Hall. The authors wish to thank the Computer Science Center of the University of Maryland for funds for data analysis.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号