首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   39799篇
  免费   1607篇
  国内免费   16篇
  41422篇
  2020年   377篇
  2019年   440篇
  2018年   592篇
  2017年   616篇
  2016年   676篇
  2015年   502篇
  2014年   583篇
  2013年   2729篇
  2012年   1120篇
  2011年   1097篇
  2010年   688篇
  2009年   660篇
  2008年   975篇
  2007年   977篇
  2006年   837篇
  2005年   812篇
  2004年   754篇
  2003年   683篇
  2002年   720篇
  2001年   1285篇
  2000年   1244篇
  1999年   907篇
  1998年   405篇
  1992年   874篇
  1991年   815篇
  1990年   829篇
  1989年   725篇
  1988年   723篇
  1987年   694篇
  1986年   725篇
  1985年   799篇
  1984年   603篇
  1983年   552篇
  1982年   376篇
  1981年   371篇
  1980年   355篇
  1979年   699篇
  1978年   436篇
  1977年   429篇
  1976年   425篇
  1975年   597篇
  1974年   687篇
  1973年   730篇
  1972年   622篇
  1971年   587篇
  1970年   576篇
  1969年   567篇
  1968年   754篇
  1967年   670篇
  1966年   598篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
2.
Previous studies showed that random error can explain overconfidence effects typically observed in the literature. One of these studies concluded that, after accounting for random error effects in the data, there is little support for cognitive‐processing biases in confidence elicitation. In this paper, we investigate more closely the random error explanation for overconfidence. We generated data from four models of confidence and then estimated the magnitude of random error in the data. Our results show that, in addition to the true magnitude of random error specified in the simulations, the error estimates are influenced by important cognitive‐processing biases in the confidence elicitation process. We found that random error in the response process can account for the degree of overconfidence found in calibration studies, even when that overconfidence is actually caused by other factors. Thus, the error models say little about whether cognitive biases are present in the confidence elicitation process. Copyright © 2008 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.  相似文献   
3.
4.
5.
The authors tested effects of a 10-week group cognitive-behavioral stress management intervention among 100 women newly treated for Stage 0-II breast cancer. The intervention reduced prevalence of moderate depression (which remained relatively stable in the control condition) but did not affect other measures of emotional distress. The intervention also increased participants' reports that having breast cancer had made positive contributions to their lives, and it increased generalized optimism. Both remained significantly elevated at a 3-month follow-up of the intervention. Further analysis revealed that the intervention had its greatest impact on these 2 variables among women who were lowest in optimism at baseline. Discussion centers on the importance of examining positive responses to traumatic events--growth, appreciation of life, shift in priorities, and positive affect-as well as negative responses.  相似文献   
6.
7.
8.
9.
10.
Using a Piagétan perspective, this study investigated the ways in which elementary school children perceive changes in the size of a televised image (in this case, a candy bar). The findings suggest that younger children perceive changes in image size from a medium shot to a close-up as changes in the object itself. Children's responses to changes in the televised image parallel their responses to traditional conservation tasks, but conservation of televised images occurs at a later age. In addition, children appear to use different cognitive skills to interpret how a zoom or a cut transforms the size of a televised image. When a zoom is used, children more readily perceive the object as “growing larger.”  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号