首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   69536篇
  免费   2645篇
  国内免费   23篇
  72204篇
  2019年   742篇
  2018年   1062篇
  2017年   1024篇
  2016年   1164篇
  2015年   858篇
  2014年   1008篇
  2013年   5737篇
  2012年   1849篇
  2011年   1852篇
  2010年   1187篇
  2009年   1129篇
  2008年   1721篇
  2007年   1618篇
  2006年   1455篇
  2005年   1318篇
  2004年   1251篇
  2003年   1210篇
  2002年   1197篇
  2001年   2185篇
  2000年   2114篇
  1999年   1577篇
  1998年   730篇
  1996年   771篇
  1995年   721篇
  1994年   727篇
  1992年   1584篇
  1991年   1449篇
  1990年   1451篇
  1989年   1283篇
  1988年   1268篇
  1987年   1253篇
  1986年   1275篇
  1985年   1314篇
  1984年   1091篇
  1983年   969篇
  1982年   709篇
  1981年   718篇
  1979年   1135篇
  1978年   787篇
  1976年   725篇
  1975年   996篇
  1974年   1078篇
  1973年   1180篇
  1972年   1063篇
  1971年   970篇
  1970年   915篇
  1969年   892篇
  1968年   1142篇
  1967年   1016篇
  1966年   924篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
2.
3.
Previous studies showed that random error can explain overconfidence effects typically observed in the literature. One of these studies concluded that, after accounting for random error effects in the data, there is little support for cognitive‐processing biases in confidence elicitation. In this paper, we investigate more closely the random error explanation for overconfidence. We generated data from four models of confidence and then estimated the magnitude of random error in the data. Our results show that, in addition to the true magnitude of random error specified in the simulations, the error estimates are influenced by important cognitive‐processing biases in the confidence elicitation process. We found that random error in the response process can account for the degree of overconfidence found in calibration studies, even when that overconfidence is actually caused by other factors. Thus, the error models say little about whether cognitive biases are present in the confidence elicitation process. Copyright © 2008 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.  相似文献   
4.
The present study explored the availability of flexible work arrangements (FWA) and their relationship with manager outcomes of job satisfaction, turnover intentions, and work‐to‐family conflict (WFC) across country clusters. We used individualism and collectivism to explain differences in FWA availability across Latin American, Anglo, and Asian clusters. Managers from the Anglo cluster were more likely to report working in organisations that offer FWA compared to managers from other clusters. For Anglo managers, flextime was the only FWA that had significant favorable relationships with the outcome variables. For Latin Americans, part‐time work negatively related with turnover intentions and strain‐based WFC. For Asians, flextime was unrelated to time‐based WFC, and telecommuting was positively associated with strain‐based WFC. The clusters did not moderate the compressed work week and outcome relationships. Implications for practitioners adopting FWA practices across cultures are discussed.  相似文献   
5.
6.
7.
8.
9.
10.
The authors tested effects of a 10-week group cognitive-behavioral stress management intervention among 100 women newly treated for Stage 0-II breast cancer. The intervention reduced prevalence of moderate depression (which remained relatively stable in the control condition) but did not affect other measures of emotional distress. The intervention also increased participants' reports that having breast cancer had made positive contributions to their lives, and it increased generalized optimism. Both remained significantly elevated at a 3-month follow-up of the intervention. Further analysis revealed that the intervention had its greatest impact on these 2 variables among women who were lowest in optimism at baseline. Discussion centers on the importance of examining positive responses to traumatic events--growth, appreciation of life, shift in priorities, and positive affect-as well as negative responses.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号