首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   71716篇
  免费   2753篇
  国内免费   25篇
  74494篇
  2019年   785篇
  2018年   1155篇
  2017年   1124篇
  2016年   1262篇
  2015年   928篇
  2014年   1087篇
  2013年   5996篇
  2012年   1986篇
  2011年   1956篇
  2010年   1261篇
  2009年   1212篇
  2008年   1819篇
  2007年   1727篇
  2006年   1523篇
  2005年   1377篇
  2004年   1302篇
  2003年   1245篇
  2002年   1246篇
  2001年   2221篇
  2000年   2157篇
  1999年   1607篇
  1998年   748篇
  1996年   784篇
  1995年   729篇
  1994年   740篇
  1993年   718篇
  1992年   1591篇
  1991年   1458篇
  1990年   1475篇
  1989年   1298篇
  1988年   1281篇
  1987年   1266篇
  1986年   1285篇
  1985年   1322篇
  1984年   1107篇
  1983年   980篇
  1981年   725篇
  1979年   1149篇
  1978年   791篇
  1976年   732篇
  1975年   1009篇
  1974年   1097篇
  1973年   1194篇
  1972年   1074篇
  1971年   979篇
  1970年   927篇
  1969年   898篇
  1968年   1152篇
  1967年   1023篇
  1966年   930篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
2.
3.
Previous studies showed that random error can explain overconfidence effects typically observed in the literature. One of these studies concluded that, after accounting for random error effects in the data, there is little support for cognitive‐processing biases in confidence elicitation. In this paper, we investigate more closely the random error explanation for overconfidence. We generated data from four models of confidence and then estimated the magnitude of random error in the data. Our results show that, in addition to the true magnitude of random error specified in the simulations, the error estimates are influenced by important cognitive‐processing biases in the confidence elicitation process. We found that random error in the response process can account for the degree of overconfidence found in calibration studies, even when that overconfidence is actually caused by other factors. Thus, the error models say little about whether cognitive biases are present in the confidence elicitation process. Copyright © 2008 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.  相似文献   
4.
The present study explored the availability of flexible work arrangements (FWA) and their relationship with manager outcomes of job satisfaction, turnover intentions, and work‐to‐family conflict (WFC) across country clusters. We used individualism and collectivism to explain differences in FWA availability across Latin American, Anglo, and Asian clusters. Managers from the Anglo cluster were more likely to report working in organisations that offer FWA compared to managers from other clusters. For Anglo managers, flextime was the only FWA that had significant favorable relationships with the outcome variables. For Latin Americans, part‐time work negatively related with turnover intentions and strain‐based WFC. For Asians, flextime was unrelated to time‐based WFC, and telecommuting was positively associated with strain‐based WFC. The clusters did not moderate the compressed work week and outcome relationships. Implications for practitioners adopting FWA practices across cultures are discussed.  相似文献   
5.
6.
7.
8.
9.
10.
Range of electric vehicles (EVs) has long been considered a major barrier in acceptance of electric mobility. We examined the nature of how range is experienced in an EV and whether variables from other adaptation contexts, notably stress, have explanatory power for inter‐individual differences in what we term comfortable range. Forty EVs were leased to a sample of users for a 6‐month field study. Qualitative and quantitative analyses of range experiences were performed, including regression analyses to examine the role of stress‐buffering personality traits and coping skills in comfortable range. Users appraised range as a resource to which they could successfully adapt and that satisfied most of their daily mobility needs. However, indicators were found that suggested suboptimal range utilisation. Stress‐buffering personality traits (control beliefs, ambiguity tolerance) and coping skills (subjective range competence, daily range practice) were found to play a substantial role in comfortable range. Hence, it may be possible to overcome perceived range barriers with the assistance of psychological interventions such as information, training, and interface design. Providing drivers with a reliable usable range may be more important than enhancing maximal range in an electric mobility system.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号