首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   208篇
  免费   15篇
  223篇
  2023年   2篇
  2020年   6篇
  2019年   4篇
  2018年   12篇
  2017年   8篇
  2016年   8篇
  2015年   6篇
  2014年   12篇
  2013年   39篇
  2012年   8篇
  2011年   6篇
  2010年   10篇
  2009年   3篇
  2008年   9篇
  2007年   7篇
  2006年   3篇
  2005年   5篇
  2004年   4篇
  2003年   7篇
  2002年   3篇
  2001年   5篇
  2000年   3篇
  1999年   1篇
  1998年   1篇
  1997年   2篇
  1996年   5篇
  1995年   3篇
  1994年   3篇
  1993年   6篇
  1992年   6篇
  1991年   1篇
  1990年   2篇
  1989年   2篇
  1988年   3篇
  1987年   2篇
  1986年   2篇
  1985年   4篇
  1984年   2篇
  1983年   2篇
  1982年   2篇
  1981年   1篇
  1980年   2篇
  1979年   1篇
排序方式: 共有223条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
2.
Much family therapy writing has focused on work with discrete presenting problems in circumscribed clinical practice settings. This paper takes a broader focus of poor families which do not fit easily into the one-hour therapy schedule, in the context of the powerful forces of politics and economics. If the prevailing political attitudes and economic realities are ignored, direct practice with families may fail to meet them at their point of experience. Examples of political views, and therapeutic and research thinking which do not pay attention to these aspects, are used to highlight the author's point of view.  相似文献   
3.
Five obliquely rotated factor analyses were performed on items from Eysenck and Eysenck’s (1975) EPQ, Zuckerman’s (1975) Sensation-Seeking Scale, and Strelau’s (1972) temperament inventory (STI), administered to 277 subjects (Ss). The analyses were used to examine the relationships between the personality dimensions E, N, L, P and Sensation-Seeking, which pertain mainly to social situations, attitudes and feelings, and questionnaire-derived measures of nervous system properties, which, while validated on experimental indices of nervous system properties derived from Nebylitsyn’s (1972) model, are expressed as characteristic features of social behavior, work style and activity. Four factors were identified—extraversion/strength of excitation/mobility; self-control of affect/stability/caution; strength of inhibition/verbal control/motor expressiveness/ nonmanipulativeness; and sensation-seeking/nervousness. The results offer support for Eysenck’s claim of some identity between excitatory strength and E, for the reported relationship between E and mobility, and for Eysenck’s conditioning postulate. They also suggest that similar temperamental variables underlie individual differences in both social and occupational/motor activity. Since Strelau’s STI items are validated on experimental indices of nervous system properties, it follows that typological characteristics may be inferred from questionnaire data derived from both behavioral categories.  相似文献   
4.
5.
Book reviews     
Hall, G. (1991). Perceptual and associative leaning. Oxfod: Clarendon Press. Pp. xi + 300. ISBN 0-19-8524182-0. £35.00.

Rahmann, H., & Rahmann, M. (1992). The neurobilogical basis of memoy and behavior. New York: Springe Verlag. Pp. 292. ISBN 3-540-97545-4. £53.00.

Iversen, I.H., & Lattal, K.A. (Eds.). (1991). Experimental analysis of behavior, arts 1 and 2. Techniques in the behavioral and neural sciences, volume 6 (series editor: J.P Huston). Amstedam: Elsevier. Pp. 724. ISBN 0-444-8932-2. £50.00 (paperback).

Baudry, M., & Davis, J.L. (Eds.) (1991). Long-tem potentiation: A debate of current issues. Cambridge, Ma & London, England: MIT Press. Pp. xxii + 454. ISBN 0-262-02330-x. £62.95 (Hb).

The Biological Council Animal Reseach and Welfare Panel (1992). Guidelines on the handling and training of laboatory animals. Univer-sities Federation for Animal Welfare. Pp. ISBN 0-900767-77-4. £2.50.  相似文献   
6.
The influence of non-reinforced exposure to compound stimuli on subsequent appetitive classical conditioning was examined in five experiments with rats as the subjects. Non-reinforced exposure to a visual stimulus retarded subsequent acquisition of conditioned responding relative to a non-pre-exposed condition (latent inhibition). If the target stimulus was pre-exposed in compound with a second (non-target) stimulus, then latent inhibition was abolished. Exposure to the non-target stimulus prior to compound exposure had varying effects on subsequent conditioning to the target: 40 exposures to the non-target stimulus resulted in latent inhibition to the target stimulus that was comparable in magnitude to that observed when the target stimulus was exposed in isolation; 120 exposures to the non-target stimulus enhanced latent inhibition to the target stimulus.  相似文献   
7.
The effects of fixed-time (FT) reinforcement schedules on the disruptive behavior of 4 students in special education classrooms were studied. Attention provided on FT schedules in the context of a multiple-baseline design across participants substantially decreased all students’ challenging behavior. Disruptive behavior was maintained at levels lower than baseline throughout reinforcement thinning and follow-up phases. The results extend existing literature on the use of FT schedules of reinforcement and provide evidence that FT schedules of reinforcement can be practical to implement in special educational classroom settings.  相似文献   
8.
9.
Stimulus over-selectivity refers to behavior being controlled by one element of the environment at the expense of other equally salient aspects of the environment. This is a common problem for many individuals, including those with autism spectrum disorders, and learning difficulties, and presents a considerable problem for information processing in many important situations involving complex cues and environments. Three experiments explored the strength and generality of the over-selectivity effect in non-clinical adults undertaking a cognitively demanding task, by training and testing participants on a two-component trial-and-error discrimination learning task. The over-selectivity effect was found in a variety of test conditions, including when the comparison test stimulus was neutral (Experiment 1), novel (Experiment 2), neutral with no conditioning history (Experiment 3), or when punished during training (Experiments 1, 2 and 3). Such results provide ubiquity to the phenomenon, making it important to investigate further.  相似文献   
10.
Memory deficits have been shown to hamper decision making in a number of populations. In two experiments, participants were required to select one of three alternatives that varied in reinforcer amount and delay, and the effect of a concurrent task on a behavioral choice task that involved making either an impulsive, self-controlled, or optimal response was studied. The studies aimed to see whether or not individuals were capable of optimizing when confronted with a three-alternative choice paradigm, and if a concurrent task could induce over-selectivity toward a self-controlled, or impulsive, choice, rather than an optimal choice. Various factors which could possibly influence optimization were also explored. The results suggest that some, but not all, individuals are capable of optimizing in a three-alternative choice paradigm, especially when there is a great differential between reinforcement rates of optimal versus non-optimal behaviors. Participants with a concurrent task (continuously subtracting seven from a large number) displayed over-selectivity, evidenced by the high proportion of self-controlled responses.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号