首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   39310篇
  免费   1619篇
  国内免费   14篇
  40943篇
  2020年   386篇
  2019年   439篇
  2018年   600篇
  2017年   611篇
  2016年   688篇
  2015年   520篇
  2014年   582篇
  2013年   2683篇
  2012年   1122篇
  2011年   1097篇
  2010年   683篇
  2009年   661篇
  2008年   969篇
  2007年   959篇
  2006年   829篇
  2005年   802篇
  2004年   745篇
  2003年   684篇
  2002年   708篇
  2001年   1270篇
  2000年   1226篇
  1999年   889篇
  1998年   397篇
  1992年   861篇
  1991年   800篇
  1990年   820篇
  1989年   714篇
  1988年   712篇
  1987年   685篇
  1986年   713篇
  1985年   787篇
  1984年   590篇
  1983年   543篇
  1982年   366篇
  1981年   356篇
  1980年   342篇
  1979年   685篇
  1978年   425篇
  1977年   415篇
  1976年   411篇
  1975年   583篇
  1974年   669篇
  1973年   713篇
  1972年   617篇
  1971年   578篇
  1970年   574篇
  1969年   561篇
  1968年   752篇
  1967年   665篇
  1966年   595篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
2.
Previous studies showed that random error can explain overconfidence effects typically observed in the literature. One of these studies concluded that, after accounting for random error effects in the data, there is little support for cognitive‐processing biases in confidence elicitation. In this paper, we investigate more closely the random error explanation for overconfidence. We generated data from four models of confidence and then estimated the magnitude of random error in the data. Our results show that, in addition to the true magnitude of random error specified in the simulations, the error estimates are influenced by important cognitive‐processing biases in the confidence elicitation process. We found that random error in the response process can account for the degree of overconfidence found in calibration studies, even when that overconfidence is actually caused by other factors. Thus, the error models say little about whether cognitive biases are present in the confidence elicitation process. Copyright © 2008 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.  相似文献   
3.
4.
5.
The purpose of this study is to compare field investigative interviews of children (FIIC) with three different legal outcomes in child sexual abuse cases: (i) insufficient evidence to proceed (IEP); (ii) convictions; or (iii) acquittals by the court. One hundred FIIC were divided into one of the three outcome possibilities. Amongst the female interviewees older than 10 years, there were no cases of acquittals and the convicted cases were over-represented. The children's response to open questions was found to be the main difference between the three FIIC outcomes. The responses to these open questions were 1.9 and 2.3 times longer in the convicted cases compared to acquittals and IEP. Possible explanations for the result are discussed. Copyright © 2009 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.  相似文献   
6.
7.
8.
9.
10.
Using a Piagétan perspective, this study investigated the ways in which elementary school children perceive changes in the size of a televised image (in this case, a candy bar). The findings suggest that younger children perceive changes in image size from a medium shot to a close-up as changes in the object itself. Children's responses to changes in the televised image parallel their responses to traditional conservation tasks, but conservation of televised images occurs at a later age. In addition, children appear to use different cognitive skills to interpret how a zoom or a cut transforms the size of a televised image. When a zoom is used, children more readily perceive the object as “growing larger.”  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号