首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   41017篇
  免费   1703篇
  国内免费   14篇
  42734篇
  2020年   414篇
  2019年   474篇
  2018年   641篇
  2017年   648篇
  2016年   726篇
  2015年   559篇
  2014年   622篇
  2013年   2857篇
  2012年   1204篇
  2011年   1167篇
  2010年   732篇
  2009年   690篇
  2008年   1049篇
  2007年   1056篇
  2006年   904篇
  2005年   875篇
  2004年   813篇
  2003年   752篇
  2002年   759篇
  2001年   1296篇
  2000年   1252篇
  1999年   917篇
  1998年   424篇
  1997年   371篇
  1992年   885篇
  1991年   819篇
  1990年   840篇
  1989年   732篇
  1988年   731篇
  1987年   699篇
  1986年   734篇
  1985年   806篇
  1984年   603篇
  1983年   558篇
  1982年   397篇
  1981年   377篇
  1979年   705篇
  1978年   438篇
  1977年   427篇
  1976年   428篇
  1975年   594篇
  1974年   682篇
  1973年   717篇
  1972年   622篇
  1971年   578篇
  1970年   577篇
  1969年   561篇
  1968年   755篇
  1967年   667篇
  1966年   597篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
2.
Previous studies showed that random error can explain overconfidence effects typically observed in the literature. One of these studies concluded that, after accounting for random error effects in the data, there is little support for cognitive‐processing biases in confidence elicitation. In this paper, we investigate more closely the random error explanation for overconfidence. We generated data from four models of confidence and then estimated the magnitude of random error in the data. Our results show that, in addition to the true magnitude of random error specified in the simulations, the error estimates are influenced by important cognitive‐processing biases in the confidence elicitation process. We found that random error in the response process can account for the degree of overconfidence found in calibration studies, even when that overconfidence is actually caused by other factors. Thus, the error models say little about whether cognitive biases are present in the confidence elicitation process. Copyright © 2008 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.  相似文献   
3.
4.
5.
This study examined individuals' tendencies to migrate from one organization to another (i.e., the propensity to switch employers). Previous researchers have suggested that switching organizations throughout the career span may be partially heritable and therefore related to individual differences in personality traits. If personality traits are indeed related to a tendency to turnover from organizations, this suggests that current procedures for calculating utility may be inaccurate. Using a database of 1081 individuals who have been in the workforce for several years, results indicated that personality traits measured by the Occupational Personality Questionnaire (non‐ipsative; OPQn) were modestly related to organization switching (i.e., repeated moves from organization to organization). We found that higher scores on extraversion, openness to experience, and conscientiousness‐related traits were modestly correlated with more frequent organization switching. However, we demonstrate that these modest relationships can produce large inaccuracies in utility estimates.  相似文献   
6.
7.
8.
9.
10.
Using a Piagétan perspective, this study investigated the ways in which elementary school children perceive changes in the size of a televised image (in this case, a candy bar). The findings suggest that younger children perceive changes in image size from a medium shot to a close-up as changes in the object itself. Children's responses to changes in the televised image parallel their responses to traditional conservation tasks, but conservation of televised images occurs at a later age. In addition, children appear to use different cognitive skills to interpret how a zoom or a cut transforms the size of a televised image. When a zoom is used, children more readily perceive the object as “growing larger.”  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号