全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2383篇 |
免费 | 38篇 |
专业分类
2421篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 26篇 |
2018年 | 29篇 |
2017年 | 32篇 |
2016年 | 38篇 |
2015年 | 23篇 |
2014年 | 35篇 |
2013年 | 281篇 |
2012年 | 71篇 |
2011年 | 64篇 |
2010年 | 45篇 |
2009年 | 62篇 |
2008年 | 85篇 |
2007年 | 95篇 |
2006年 | 94篇 |
2005年 | 76篇 |
2004年 | 69篇 |
2003年 | 70篇 |
2002年 | 55篇 |
2001年 | 41篇 |
2000年 | 46篇 |
1999年 | 42篇 |
1998年 | 37篇 |
1997年 | 38篇 |
1996年 | 31篇 |
1995年 | 30篇 |
1994年 | 28篇 |
1993年 | 22篇 |
1992年 | 41篇 |
1991年 | 33篇 |
1990年 | 19篇 |
1989年 | 22篇 |
1988年 | 21篇 |
1987年 | 28篇 |
1986年 | 28篇 |
1985年 | 33篇 |
1984年 | 32篇 |
1983年 | 34篇 |
1982年 | 41篇 |
1981年 | 40篇 |
1980年 | 30篇 |
1979年 | 27篇 |
1978年 | 36篇 |
1977年 | 32篇 |
1975年 | 28篇 |
1974年 | 25篇 |
1973年 | 27篇 |
1971年 | 27篇 |
1970年 | 19篇 |
1969年 | 20篇 |
1968年 | 18篇 |
排序方式: 共有2421条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
2.
3.
4.
Jonathan Grainger J. Kevin O’Regan Arthur M. Jacobs Juan Segui 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1992,51(1):49-56
Two experiments are described that measured lexical decision latencies and errors to five-letter French words with a single higher frequency orthographic neighbor and control words with no higher frequency neighbors. The higher frequency neighbor differed from the stimulus word by either the second letter (e.g.,astre-autre) or the fourth letter (chope-chose). Neighborhood frequency effects were found to interact with this factor, and significant interference was observed only tochope-type words. The effects of neighborhood frequency were also found to interact with the position of initial fixation in the stimulus word (either the second letter or the fourth letter). Interference was greatly reduced when the initial fixation was on the critical disambiguating letter (i.e., the letterp inchope). Moreover, word recognition was improved when subjects initially fixated the second letter relative to when they initially fixated the fourth letter of a five-letter word, but this second-letter advantage practically disappeared when the stimulus differed from a more frequent word by its fourth letter. The results are interpreted in terms of the interaction between visual and lexical factors in visual word recognition. 相似文献
5.
Albert Ellis Joyce Sichel Russell C. Leaf Roslyn Mass 《Journal of Rational-Emotive & Cognitive-Behavior Therapy》1989,7(4):197-218
The failure of controlled experimental research on therapy out-come to produce findings of clinical as well as statistical significance seems at least partly due to irrational, perfectionistic, beliefs about how research can be used to infer causes of therapeutic change. Irrational beliefs about controlled experimental designs and about incomplete sampling, in particular, appear to prevent appropriate consideration of probatively valuable quasi-experimental research. Models of more rational alternatives are identified, and an example of the functional advantages of adopting these rational beliefs is explicated. The example demonstrates that single-shot pre-post survey data from self-selected catch-as-catch-can samples can permit legitimate, but not absolutely certain, conclusions about the efficacy and efficiency of RET.Albert Ellis, Ph.D. is executive director of the Institute for Rational-Emotive Therapy in New York City. 相似文献
6.
Carl E. Granrud Albert Yonas Elizabeth A. Opland 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1985,37(5):415-419
Five- and 7-month-old infants were tested for sensitivity to the depth cue of shading. Infants were presented with two displays: a surface in which a convexity and a concavity were molded and a photograph in which shading specified a convexity and a concavity. Each display was presented under both monocular and binocular viewing conditions. Reaching was observed as the dependent measure. Infants in both age groups reached preferentially for the actual convexity in both the monocular and binocular viewing conditions. In the monocular photograph condition, the 7-month-olds reached preferentially for the apparent convexity specified by shading, indicating that they perceived it to be an actual convexity. These infants showed no significant reaching preference in the binocular photograph condition. This finding rules out interpretations of the infants’ reaching not based on perceived depth. The results therefore suggest that the 7-month-olds perceived depth from shading. The 5-month-olds showed no significant reaching preferences when viewing the photograph; thus, they showed no evidence of depth perception from shading. These findings are consistent with the results of a number of studies that have investigated infants’ sensitivity to pictorial depth cues. Together, these studies suggest that the ability to perceive depth from pictorial cues may first develop between 5 and 7 months of age. 相似文献
7.
8.
A Z Arthur 《Perceptual and motor skills》1987,64(1):75-85
The pituitary-adrenocortical system is activated in stressful situations, and the adrenocortical hormones have been implicated by Selye in the diseases of stress. Evidence indicates that the adrenocortical hormones peak during the period of anticipation of stressful events, not during actual confrontation with them. The circadian cortisol rhythm shows peaks before waking, not during active periods of the day, and cortisol does not appear to be required during physical activity. The role of these hormones appears to be to suppress ongoing physiological activity in order to increase vigilance and to get the organism ready to take action before the impact of dangers. 相似文献
9.
Ricks Warren Robert W. McLellarn Albert Ellis Ph.D. 《Journal of Rational-Emotive & Cognitive-Behavior Therapy》1987,5(2):92-107
This article presents Albert Ellis' personal responses to the Survey of Rational-Emotive Therapists and his answers to a number of questions that the first and second authors asked Ellis regarding some of his responses. Ellis also commented on how adequately he thought the results of the survey appeared to represent the current philosophies and practices of RET. 相似文献
10.
Patricia B. Sutker Daniel K. Winstead Kenneth C. Goist Robert M. Malow Albert N. Allain Jr. 《Journal of psychopathology and behavioral assessment》1986,8(2):89-101
Psychopathology and symptom patterns were studied in 60 former prisoners-of-war (POWs) by administering standardized tests including the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI), an adjustment problem checklist, and a structured clinical interview. Most POWs showed marked psychological impairment, but modal profile analysis identified two prototypic MMPI patterns, which differed in pervasiveness and type of psychopathology. Profile subtypes were defined by unique clusters of clinical symptoms and differed in confinement stress severity. The typology of symptoms argues against a homogeneous conceptualization of stress-induced disorders and suggests the need for definition of the severity and subtype of stress phenomena and individual difference factors in responding to trauma. 相似文献