首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   34373篇
  免费   1139篇
  国内免费   12篇
  35524篇
  2019年   303篇
  2018年   540篇
  2017年   548篇
  2016年   639篇
  2015年   444篇
  2014年   467篇
  2013年   2311篇
  2012年   1027篇
  2011年   1071篇
  2010年   571篇
  2009年   530篇
  2008年   782篇
  2007年   712篇
  2006年   703篇
  2005年   1814篇
  2004年   1180篇
  2003年   980篇
  2002年   710篇
  2001年   840篇
  2000年   826篇
  1999年   660篇
  1998年   307篇
  1997年   317篇
  1996年   316篇
  1992年   608篇
  1991年   584篇
  1990年   588篇
  1989年   536篇
  1988年   534篇
  1987年   520篇
  1986年   569篇
  1985年   538篇
  1984年   474篇
  1983年   414篇
  1982年   295篇
  1981年   315篇
  1979年   523篇
  1978年   380篇
  1977年   330篇
  1976年   352篇
  1975年   432篇
  1974年   548篇
  1973年   571篇
  1972年   437篇
  1971年   458篇
  1970年   411篇
  1969年   450篇
  1968年   516篇
  1967年   490篇
  1966年   478篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
2.
3.
In two-choice tasks, the compatible mapping of left stimulus to left response and right stimulus to right response typically yields better performance than does the incompatible mapping. Nonetheless, when compatible and incompatible mappings are mixed within a block of trials, the spatial compatibility effect is eliminated. Two experiments evaluated whether the elimination of compatibility effects by mixing compatible and incompatible mappings is a general or specific phenomenon. Left-right physical locations, arrow directions, and location words were mapped to keypress responses in Experiment 1 and vocal responses in Experiment 2. With keypresses, mixing compatible and incompatible mappings eliminated the compatibility effect for physical locations and arrow directions, but enhanced it for words. With vocal responses, mixing significantly reduced the compatibility effect only for words. Overall, the mixing effects suggest that elimination or reduction of compatibility effects occurs primarily when the stimulus-response sets have both conceptual and perceptual similarity. This elimination may be due to suppression of a direct response-selection route, but to account for the full pattern of mixing effects it is also necessary to consider changes in an indirect response-selection route and the temporal activation properties of different stimulus-response sets.  相似文献   
4.
5.
6.
7.
8.
9.
Cognitive neuropsychology provides a theoretical framework and methods that can be of value in the study of developmental disorders, but the "dissociation" logic at the centre of this approach is not well suited to the developmental context. This is illustrated with examples from specific language impairment. Within the developing language system there is ample evidence for interaction between levels of representation, with modularity emerging in the course of development. This means that one typically is seeking to explain a complex pattern of associated impairments, rather than highly selective deficits. For instance, a selective impairment in auditory processing can have repercussions through the language system and may lead to distinctive syntactic deficits that are seen in written as well as spoken language. Changes in the nature of representations and in the relationships between components of a developing system mean that cross-sectional data at a single point in development may be misleading indicators of the primary deficit. Furthermore, traditional cognitive neuropsychology places a disproportionate emphasis on representational (competence) deficits, with processing (performance) deficits being relatively neglected. Methods for distinguishing these two kinds of impairment are discussed, as well as other approaches for elucidating the underlying nature of developmental disorders.  相似文献   
10.
We investigated eye‐movements during preschool children's pictorial recall of seen objects. Thirteen 3‐ to 4‐year‐old children completed a perceptual encoding and a pictorial recall task. First, they were exposed to 16 pictorial objects, which were positioned in one of four distinct areas on the computer screen. Subsequently, they had to recall these pictorial objects from memory in order to respond to specific questions about visual details. We found that children spent more time fixating the areas in which the pictorial objects were previously displayed. We conclude that as early as age 3–4 years old, children show specific eye‐movements when they recall pictorial contents of previously seen objects.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号