全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1682篇 |
免费 | 15篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
1698篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 73篇 |
2023年 | 8篇 |
2022年 | 8篇 |
2021年 | 18篇 |
2020年 | 164篇 |
2019年 | 212篇 |
2018年 | 33篇 |
2017年 | 35篇 |
2016年 | 216篇 |
2015年 | 211篇 |
2014年 | 44篇 |
2013年 | 92篇 |
2012年 | 36篇 |
2011年 | 37篇 |
2010年 | 25篇 |
2009年 | 18篇 |
2008年 | 38篇 |
2007年 | 29篇 |
2006年 | 23篇 |
2005年 | 20篇 |
2004年 | 25篇 |
2003年 | 29篇 |
2002年 | 28篇 |
2001年 | 8篇 |
2000年 | 14篇 |
1999年 | 15篇 |
1998年 | 11篇 |
1997年 | 11篇 |
1996年 | 5篇 |
1995年 | 5篇 |
1994年 | 15篇 |
1993年 | 6篇 |
1992年 | 13篇 |
1991年 | 7篇 |
1990年 | 8篇 |
1989年 | 9篇 |
1988年 | 8篇 |
1986年 | 12篇 |
1985年 | 7篇 |
1984年 | 9篇 |
1983年 | 13篇 |
1982年 | 9篇 |
1981年 | 7篇 |
1980年 | 6篇 |
1978年 | 8篇 |
1977年 | 10篇 |
1976年 | 7篇 |
1975年 | 6篇 |
1973年 | 7篇 |
1966年 | 6篇 |
排序方式: 共有1698条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
Jeffrey S. Levin Ph.D. M.P.H. C. David Jenkins Ph.D. Robert M. Rose M. D. 《Journal of religion and health》1988,27(4):267-278
In a study of air traffic controllers, religious differences are found in the way Type A behavior is associated with several health status indicators. Associations between the Jenkins Activity Survey (JAS) and physical illness incidence, health-promotive behavior, diastolic and systolic blood pressure, subjective distress and impulse control problems, and alcohol consumption are examined by religious attendance, religious affiliation, and change in affiliation. Findings confirm that Type A does not vary significantly by religion. However, there are several significant findings between Type A and various health indicators. Type A is associated with illness incidence, overall and more strongly in several religion, subgroups. Type A and alcohol consumption are related positively in Protestants and converts, and negatively in churchgoing Catholics. Type A is related to impulse control problems in churchgoing Protestants and to subjective distress in churchgoing Catholics. Finally, in individuals with weak or no religious ties, Type A is associated with lower blood pressure. This last finding suggests that in some people (for example, the irreligious or unchurched), the coronary-prone behavior pattern may have cardiovascular effects which are salutary in at least one respect.The authors wish to thank Dr. Kyriakos S. Markides and Laura A. Ray for their assistance with this study. Address requests for reprints to Dr. Levin at the Institute of Gerontology, 300 North Ingalls, Ann Arbor, MI 48109. 相似文献
2.
This paper contrasts the basic tenets of systemic thinking with some guiding principles of the psychodynamic approach, and outlines specific techniques which family therapists can use when seeing individuals. It is argued that a useful systemic framework can be maintained if the therapist aims to keep the therapy system 'open' for relevant others to join at any time. 相似文献
3.
4.
Valerie A. Hartouni 《希帕蒂亚:女权主义哲学杂志》1986,1(1):3-20
I would like to thank Donna Haraway, Barrie Thome, Daniel Scripture and, especially, Peter Euben for their many helpful comments and suggestions on various drafts of this article. I would also like to thank Thelma Francis for her generous support and encouragement. 相似文献
5.
Generalization gradients of inhibition following auditory discrimination learning 总被引:3,自引:3,他引:0
下载免费PDF全文

A more direct method than the usual ones for obtaining inhibitory gradients requires that the dimension of the nonreinforced stimulus selected for testing be orthogonal to the dimensions of the reinforced stimulus. In that case, the test points along the inhibitory gradient are equally distant from the reinforced stimulus. An attempt was made to realize this condition by obtaining inhibitory gradients along the frequency dimension of a pure tone after discrimination training in which the nonreinforced stimulus was a pure tone (or tones), and the reinforced stimulus was either white noise or the absence of a tone. The results showed that some degree of specific inhibitory control was exerted by the frequency of the tone, although the gradients were broad and shallow in slope.
A further experiment was conducted to see whether the modification of an excitatory gradient resulting from training to discriminate neighboring tones could arise from a simple interaction of inhibitory and excitatory gradients. The results indicated that it could not, since discrimination training produced a concentration of responding in the vicinity of the reinforced stimulus which cannot be derived from any plausible gradient of inhibition.
相似文献6.
7.
Differential memory for picture and word stimuli 总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4
8.
Hugh Jenkins B. Phil. 《Contemporary Family Therapy》1990,12(4):311-325
Much family therapy writing has focused on work with discrete presenting problems in circumscribed clinical practice settings. This paper takes a broader focus of poor families which do not fit easily into the one-hour therapy schedule, in the context of the powerful forces of politics and economics. If the prevailing political attitudes and economic realities are ignored, direct practice with families may fail to meet them at their point of experience. Examples of political views, and therapeutic and research thinking which do not pay attention to these aspects, are used to highlight the author's point of view. 相似文献
9.
When an observer views a moving scene binocularly, both motion parallax and binocular disparity provide depth information. In Experiments lA-1C, we measured sensitivity to surface curvature when these depth cues were available either individually or simultaneously. When the depth cues yielded comparable sensitivity to surface curvature, we found that curvature detection was easier with the cues present simultaneously, rather than individually. For 2 of the 6 subjects, this effect was stronger when the component of frontal translation of the surface was vertical, rather than horizontal. No such anisotropy was found for the 4 other subjects. If a moving object is observed binocularly, the patterns of optic flow are different on the left and right retinae. We have suggested elsewhere (Cornilleau-Pérès & Droulez, in press) that this motion disparity might be used as avisual cue for the perception of a 3-D structure. Our model consisted in deriving binocular disparity from the left and right distributions of vertical velocities, rather than from luminous intensities, as has been done in classical studies on stereoscopic vision. The model led to some predictions concerning the detection of surface curvature from motion disparity in the presence or absence of intensity-based disparity (classically termedbinocular disparity). In a second set of experiments, we attempted to test these predictions, and we failed to validate our theoretical scheme from a physiological point of view. 相似文献
10.
Nancy A. Neef Jeffrey Lensbower Idil Hockersmith Valerie DePalma Kevin Gray 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》1990,23(4):447-458
We analyzed the role of the range of variation in training exemplars as a contextual variable influencing the effects of in vivo versus simulation training in producing generalized responding. Four mentally retarded adults received single case instruction, followed by general case instruction, on washing machine and dryer use; one task was taught using actual appliances (in vivo) and the other using simulation. In vivo and simulation training were counterbalanced across the two tasks for the 2 subject pairs, using a within-subjects Latin square design. With both paradigms, more errors were made after single case than after general case instruction during probe sessions with untrained washing machines and dryers. These results suggest that generalization errors were affected by the range of training exemplars and not by the use of simulated versus natural training stimuli. Although both general case simulation and general case in vivo training facilitated generalized performance of laundry skills, an analysis of training time and costs indicated that the former approach was more efficient. The study illustrates a methodology for studying complex interactions and guiding decisions on the optimal use of instructional alternatives. 相似文献