全文获取类型
收费全文 | 3847篇 |
免费 | 84篇 |
国内免费 | 7篇 |
专业分类
3938篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 5篇 |
2015年 | 4篇 |
2012年 | 454篇 |
2011年 | 482篇 |
2010年 | 91篇 |
2009年 | 57篇 |
2008年 | 390篇 |
2007年 | 404篇 |
2006年 | 372篇 |
2005年 | 288篇 |
2004年 | 261篇 |
2003年 | 240篇 |
2002年 | 206篇 |
2001年 | 131篇 |
2000年 | 182篇 |
1999年 | 78篇 |
1998年 | 17篇 |
1997年 | 12篇 |
1996年 | 7篇 |
1995年 | 7篇 |
1994年 | 4篇 |
1993年 | 6篇 |
1992年 | 8篇 |
1991年 | 13篇 |
1990年 | 4篇 |
1989年 | 11篇 |
1988年 | 8篇 |
1987年 | 8篇 |
1986年 | 8篇 |
1985年 | 9篇 |
1984年 | 5篇 |
1983年 | 7篇 |
1981年 | 5篇 |
1980年 | 7篇 |
1978年 | 12篇 |
1977年 | 4篇 |
1976年 | 6篇 |
1974年 | 4篇 |
1973年 | 5篇 |
1972年 | 6篇 |
1965年 | 5篇 |
1963年 | 6篇 |
1959年 | 6篇 |
1958年 | 20篇 |
1957年 | 13篇 |
1956年 | 11篇 |
1955年 | 7篇 |
1952年 | 5篇 |
1950年 | 5篇 |
1948年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有3938条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
Stokes T 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》1992,25(2):429-432
2.
Three rats earned their daily food ration by responding during individual trials either on a lever that delivered one food pellet immediately or on a second lever that delivered three pellets after a delay that was continuously adjusted to ensure substantial responding to both alternatives. Choice of the delayed reinforcer increased when the number of trials per session was reduced. This result suggests that models seeking closure on choice effects must include a parameter reflecting how preference changes with sessionwide income. Moreover, models positing that reinforcer probability and immediacy (1/delay) function equivalently in choice are called into question by the finding that probability and immediacy produce opposing effects when income level is changed. 相似文献
3.
4.
5.
McIntire KD Cleary J Thompson T 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》1987,47(3):279-285
Two cynomolgous macaques categorized six colors into two groups of three after conditional discrimination training (zero-delay symbolic match-to-sample). The procedures resulted in the establishment of relations among the elements of each set-relations that were not specifically trained and that can be characterized by the properties of reflexivity, symmetry, and transitivity. Each set of colors was related to a characteristic pattern of responding: One response pattern involved temporal duration (press and hold the response keys); the second response pattern entailed repeated pressing and releasing of the response keys (fixed ratio 8). Six combinations of two colors were trained, three combinations from each set. After discriminative performance stabilized for each monkey, they were tested with 10 additional color combinations, all of which differed from the training combinations. The conditional relations established between test combinations can be characterized as stimulus equivalence. The training procedures were analogous to the procedure of using category names, and have implications for understanding the function of language in the formation of equivalence classes. 相似文献
6.
It is suggested that molar streams of behavior are constructed of various arrangements of three elementary constituents (elicited, evoked, and emitted response classes). An eight-cell taxonomy is elaborated as a framework for analyzing and synthesizing complex behavioral repertoires based on these functional units. It is proposed that the local force binding functional units into a smoothly articulated kinetic sequence arises from temporally arranged relative response probability relationships. Behavioral integration is thought to reflect the joint influence of the organism's hierarchy of relative response probabilities, fluctuating biological states, and the arrangement of environmental and behavioral events in time. 相似文献
7.
8.
9.
Three students with moderate disabilities were taught to read and match-to-sample sight words comprising stimulus sets based upon the four food groups. We taught students conditional discriminations within four four-member sets using a single-sample/four-comparison procedure. Students were taught A-B, B-C, and C-D conditional discriminations for each of the four potential stimulus classes. Subsequent probes tested for relations based upon symmetry and one-node and two-node transitivity. The performances for all students indicated that symmetric relations emerged before one-node transitive relations, and that one-node transitive relations emerged before two-node transitive relations. These results are consistent with a pattern of responding, referred to as a "nodality effect," in which relations with fewer nodes are demonstrated prior to the demonstration of relations with a greater number of nodes. These results extend this area of research to sight-word reading for students with moderate disabilities. 相似文献
10.