首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   66774篇
  免费   2699篇
  国内免费   20篇
  69493篇
  2020年   615篇
  2019年   787篇
  2018年   1068篇
  2017年   1101篇
  2016年   1152篇
  2015年   857篇
  2014年   1021篇
  2013年   4758篇
  2012年   1948篇
  2011年   1978篇
  2010年   1209篇
  2009年   1168篇
  2008年   1748篇
  2007年   1722篇
  2006年   1562篇
  2005年   1455篇
  2004年   1382篇
  2003年   1266篇
  2002年   1295篇
  2001年   2068篇
  2000年   1929篇
  1999年   1468篇
  1998年   731篇
  1996年   607篇
  1992年   1312篇
  1991年   1233篇
  1990年   1216篇
  1989年   1140篇
  1988年   1149篇
  1987年   1095篇
  1986年   1133篇
  1985年   1231篇
  1984年   952篇
  1983年   902篇
  1982年   687篇
  1981年   654篇
  1979年   1114篇
  1978年   752篇
  1977年   681篇
  1976年   719篇
  1975年   938篇
  1974年   1066篇
  1973年   1130篇
  1972年   953篇
  1971年   878篇
  1970年   878篇
  1969年   894篇
  1968年   1141篇
  1967年   1013篇
  1966年   936篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
2.
Previous studies showed that random error can explain overconfidence effects typically observed in the literature. One of these studies concluded that, after accounting for random error effects in the data, there is little support for cognitive‐processing biases in confidence elicitation. In this paper, we investigate more closely the random error explanation for overconfidence. We generated data from four models of confidence and then estimated the magnitude of random error in the data. Our results show that, in addition to the true magnitude of random error specified in the simulations, the error estimates are influenced by important cognitive‐processing biases in the confidence elicitation process. We found that random error in the response process can account for the degree of overconfidence found in calibration studies, even when that overconfidence is actually caused by other factors. Thus, the error models say little about whether cognitive biases are present in the confidence elicitation process. Copyright © 2008 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.  相似文献   
3.
The present study explored the availability of flexible work arrangements (FWA) and their relationship with manager outcomes of job satisfaction, turnover intentions, and work‐to‐family conflict (WFC) across country clusters. We used individualism and collectivism to explain differences in FWA availability across Latin American, Anglo, and Asian clusters. Managers from the Anglo cluster were more likely to report working in organisations that offer FWA compared to managers from other clusters. For Anglo managers, flextime was the only FWA that had significant favorable relationships with the outcome variables. For Latin Americans, part‐time work negatively related with turnover intentions and strain‐based WFC. For Asians, flextime was unrelated to time‐based WFC, and telecommuting was positively associated with strain‐based WFC. The clusters did not moderate the compressed work week and outcome relationships. Implications for practitioners adopting FWA practices across cultures are discussed.  相似文献   
4.
5.
6.
7.
8.
9.
In this introduction to the special issue on teaching about gender and ethnicity in psychology, we consider the assumptions underlying an inclusive curriculum that pays attention to gender and ethnicity and address why such a curriculum has merit. We review empirical studies, assessing whether existing curricula are inclusive, and present an overview of the articles and the recurrent themes. These themes include the complexity of the interaction between ethnicity and gender; the difficulty of deciding which of the many possible ethnic groups to include in course material; the dominance of evaluative comparison in discussions of differences among groups; the interdisciplinary nature of research on ethnicity; and the tendency in psychology to ignore the importance of the power differences that confound analyses of the effect of ethnicity and gender.  相似文献   
10.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号