全文获取类型
收费全文 | 3535篇 |
免费 | 203篇 |
国内免费 | 32篇 |
专业分类
3770篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 57篇 |
2022年 | 48篇 |
2021年 | 53篇 |
2020年 | 108篇 |
2019年 | 113篇 |
2018年 | 141篇 |
2017年 | 142篇 |
2016年 | 153篇 |
2015年 | 85篇 |
2014年 | 120篇 |
2013年 | 390篇 |
2012年 | 175篇 |
2011年 | 195篇 |
2010年 | 122篇 |
2009年 | 119篇 |
2008年 | 130篇 |
2007年 | 158篇 |
2006年 | 133篇 |
2005年 | 111篇 |
2004年 | 105篇 |
2003年 | 95篇 |
2002年 | 98篇 |
2001年 | 74篇 |
2000年 | 47篇 |
1999年 | 41篇 |
1998年 | 32篇 |
1997年 | 28篇 |
1996年 | 27篇 |
1995年 | 34篇 |
1994年 | 42篇 |
1993年 | 24篇 |
1992年 | 37篇 |
1991年 | 28篇 |
1990年 | 25篇 |
1989年 | 26篇 |
1988年 | 35篇 |
1987年 | 21篇 |
1986年 | 28篇 |
1985年 | 31篇 |
1984年 | 24篇 |
1983年 | 20篇 |
1982年 | 21篇 |
1981年 | 15篇 |
1979年 | 24篇 |
1978年 | 31篇 |
1977年 | 20篇 |
1976年 | 15篇 |
1974年 | 26篇 |
1973年 | 18篇 |
1972年 | 15篇 |
排序方式: 共有3770条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
Eun‐Ju Lee 《人类交流研究》2004,30(2):234-259
Two experiments investigated if and how visual representation of interactants affects depersonalization and conformity to group norms in anonymous computer‐mediated communication (CMC). In Experiment 1, a 2 (intergroup versus interpersonal) × 2 (same character versus different character) between‐subjects design experiment (N= 60), each participant made a decision about social dilemmas after seeing two other (ostensible) participants’ unanimous opinions and then exchanged supporting arguments. Consistent with the Social Identity model of Deindividuation Effects (SIDE), when the group level of self‐identity was rendered salient in an intergroup encounter, uniform virtual appearance of CMC partners triggered depersonalization and subsequent conformity behavior. By contrast, when the personal dimension of the self was salient, standardized representation tended to reduce conformity. To elucidate the mediation process, Experiment 2 investigated the causal links between depersonalization, group identification, and conformity. The results show that depersonalization accentuated adherence to group norms, both directly and indirectly via group identification. 相似文献
2.
Kwan Min Lee 《人类交流研究》2004,30(2):182-207
Two experiments examine the effect of multiple synthetic voices in an e‐commerce context. In Study 1, participants (N= 40) heard five positive reviews about a book from five different synthetic voices or from a single synthetic voice. Consistent with the multiple source effect, results showed that participants hearing multiple synthetic voices evaluated the reviewed books more positively, predicted more favorable public reaction to the books, and felt greater social presence of virtual speakers. The effects were mediated by participants’ feelings of social presence. The second experiment (N= 40) showed that the observed effects persisted even when participants were shown the purely artificial nature of synthesized speech. These results support the idea that characteristics of synthetic voices in doubly disembodied language settings influence participants’ imagination of virtual speakers, and that technological literacy does not hinder social responses to anthropomorphic technologies such as text‐to‐speech (TTS). 相似文献
3.
4.
5.
V Di Lollo P Dixon 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》1988,14(4):671-681
Iconic memory, which was initially regarded as a unitary phenomenon, has since been subdivided into several components. In the present work we examined the joint effects of two such components (visible persistence and the visual analog representation) on performance in a partial report task. The display consisted of 15 alphabetic characters arranged around the perimeter of an imaginary circle on the face of an oscilloscope. The observer named the character singled out by a bar-probe. Two factors were varied: exposure duration of the array (10, 50, 100, 150, 200, 300, 400 or 500 ms) and duration of blank period (interstimulus interval, ISI) between the termination of the array and the onset of the probe (0, 50, 100, 150, or 200 ms). Performance was progressively impaired as both exposure duration and ISI were increased. The results were explained in terms of a probabilistic combinatorial model in which the timecourses of visible persistence and of the visual analog representation are regarded as time-locked to the onset and to the end of stimulation, respectively. The impairing effect of exposure duration was attributed to the relatively high spatial demands of the task that could be met optimally by information in visible persistence (which declines as a function of exposure duration), but less adequately by information in the visual analog representation. A second experiment, employing a task with lesser spatial demands, confirmed this interpretation. 相似文献
6.
7.
Gordon W. Russell Sherry L. Di Lullo Dany Di Lullo 《Current psychology (New Brunswick, N.J.)》1988,7(4):312-321
Males were randomly assigned to view either (1) a film clip featuring hockey fights or (2) a film of nonaggressive hockey
action or (3) a no-film control condition after having first been angered or treated politely by an experimental confederate.
The dependent variable was represented by a measure of aggressive mood and a behavioral measure of retaliatory aggression.
Analyses revealed that both angered and nonangered subjects exhibited an increase in aggressive mood following exposure to
the fight film. However, the analysis involving retaliatory aggression against the confederate yielded an anger x film interaction.
While angered subjects were more aggressive than nonangered, only angered subjects retaliated against the confederate after
viewing the fight film. The results were discussed in terms of Berkowitz’s (1974) aggressive cue theory.
A version of this paper was presented at the meeting of the North American Society for the Sociology of Sport, Edmonton, Canada,
November 1987. 相似文献
8.
9.
10.