全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1052篇 |
免费 | 7篇 |
专业分类
1059篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 8篇 |
2020年 | 11篇 |
2019年 | 19篇 |
2018年 | 26篇 |
2017年 | 22篇 |
2016年 | 33篇 |
2015年 | 13篇 |
2014年 | 25篇 |
2013年 | 119篇 |
2012年 | 51篇 |
2011年 | 56篇 |
2010年 | 26篇 |
2009年 | 38篇 |
2008年 | 53篇 |
2007年 | 43篇 |
2006年 | 35篇 |
2005年 | 45篇 |
2004年 | 23篇 |
2003年 | 37篇 |
2002年 | 26篇 |
2001年 | 21篇 |
2000年 | 18篇 |
1999年 | 13篇 |
1998年 | 16篇 |
1997年 | 6篇 |
1996年 | 7篇 |
1995年 | 10篇 |
1994年 | 6篇 |
1992年 | 12篇 |
1991年 | 18篇 |
1990年 | 11篇 |
1989年 | 15篇 |
1988年 | 14篇 |
1987年 | 9篇 |
1986年 | 10篇 |
1985年 | 7篇 |
1984年 | 12篇 |
1983年 | 9篇 |
1982年 | 8篇 |
1981年 | 16篇 |
1979年 | 10篇 |
1978年 | 6篇 |
1977年 | 8篇 |
1975年 | 8篇 |
1974年 | 8篇 |
1973年 | 9篇 |
1972年 | 10篇 |
1971年 | 6篇 |
1967年 | 7篇 |
1966年 | 6篇 |
排序方式: 共有1059条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
Robert L. Beck 《Journal of Family Therapy》1989,11(3):231-241
This paper describes the use of concurrent individual and conjoint sessions in couples psychotherapy. The introduction of these individual interviews is seen as supporting the individual changes which must be made to effect change in the structure of the relationship. The benefits of this interventive scheme in dynamically oriented couples' treatment will be illustrated through the presentation of case examples. 相似文献
2.
This paper addresses the use of the weekly written summary with patients in group psychotherapy. In an effort to stimulate group process and effect cotherapist communication, the authors instituted this group recording in an ongoing psychodynamically oriented mixed adult group. While the utilization of this device did, in fact, stimulate the group process and became an important part of the group's life, the therapists additionally saw significant effects upon their working alliance. These included increased focused dialogue upon group dynamics, more purposeful planning for group interventions, and heightened awareness of the significance of interventions in the group.Ms. Bosman-Clark is formerly Clinical Instructor, Department of Psychiatry, Baylor College of Medicine. 相似文献
3.
Two experiments demonstrate a positivity bias in person memory. Recall is superior for statements endorsed by a target person than for denied statements. This effect of informational positivity is independent of affective positivity (Experiment 1) and on holds for statements associated with one individual as an organizing category (Experiment 2). 相似文献
4.
Ann R. Beck 《Brain and language》1984,22(2):320-338
The ability of aphasic subjects to process the sounds and meanings of ongoing speech was tested. Subjects, 10 aphasic and 20 control, heard test sentences which contained one member of phonemically similar word pairs in one of three semantic contexts: congruent, neutral, and noncongruent. Immediately after hearing a sentence, subjects were to indicate which member of the word pair had been in the sentence. All subject groups had similar overall patterns of response to the different semantic contexts. The division of aphasic subjects into groups of high and low comprehenders revealed response differences. High comprehending aphasic subjects, like control subjects, demonstrated interactive processing of the sounds and meaning of speech while low comprehending aphasic subjects did not. 相似文献
5.
6.
W. Trammell Neill L. Scott Lissner Jean L. Beck 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1990,48(4):398-400
Responses to recently ignored information may be slower or less accurate than responses to information not recently encountered. Such negative priming effects imply that the mechanism of selective attention operates on unattended, as well as attended, information. In the present experiment, subjects judged the second and fourth letters of five-letter strings (e.g., BABAB) as “same” or “different.” Responses were slower when a target letter was identical to the distractors presented in the immediately preceding trial. This effect did not depend on which response was required on the current or preceding trial. The results suggest that ignored information is functionally disconnected from the response system as a whole, rather than from a specific response. 相似文献
7.
8.
9.
10.
Marek Franěk Jiři Mates Tomáš Radil Karin Beck Ernst Pöppel 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1994,55(2):204-217
Musically trained and untrained subjects (N=30) were asked to synchronize their finger tapping with stimuli in auditory patterns. Each pattern comprised six successive tonal stimuli of the same duration, the first of which was accented by a different frequency. The duration of interstimulus onset intervals (ISIs) gradually increased or decreased in constant steps toward the end of the patterns. Four values of such steps were used in different trials: 20, 30, 45, and 60 msec. Various time-control mechanisms are hypothesized as being simultaneously responsible for subjects’ incorrect reproduction of the internal temporal ratios of the stimulus patterns. The mechanism of assimilation (of a central tendency) led subjects to enforce a regular (isochronous) structure on the patterns. The influence of other time-control mechanisms (distinction, subjective expression of an accent, sequential transfer) was expressed mainly in differences between intertap onset intervals (ITIs) and the corresponding ISIs at the beginning of the patterns. The duration of the first two ITIs was in the majority of the trials in an inverse ratio to the ratio of the respective ISIs. The distortions resulting from the timing mechanisms concerned were more pronounced in the performance of nonmusicians than in that of musicians. 相似文献