全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2701篇 |
免费 | 142篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
2845篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 16篇 |
2021年 | 29篇 |
2020年 | 59篇 |
2019年 | 63篇 |
2018年 | 68篇 |
2017年 | 80篇 |
2016年 | 80篇 |
2015年 | 60篇 |
2014年 | 78篇 |
2013年 | 304篇 |
2012年 | 117篇 |
2011年 | 132篇 |
2010年 | 93篇 |
2009年 | 84篇 |
2008年 | 99篇 |
2007年 | 122篇 |
2006年 | 113篇 |
2005年 | 98篇 |
2004年 | 89篇 |
2003年 | 98篇 |
2002年 | 88篇 |
2001年 | 39篇 |
2000年 | 28篇 |
1999年 | 38篇 |
1998年 | 34篇 |
1997年 | 40篇 |
1996年 | 35篇 |
1995年 | 39篇 |
1994年 | 29篇 |
1993年 | 34篇 |
1992年 | 30篇 |
1991年 | 35篇 |
1990年 | 23篇 |
1989年 | 23篇 |
1988年 | 27篇 |
1987年 | 18篇 |
1986年 | 21篇 |
1985年 | 13篇 |
1983年 | 17篇 |
1982年 | 22篇 |
1981年 | 27篇 |
1980年 | 18篇 |
1979年 | 20篇 |
1978年 | 26篇 |
1977年 | 27篇 |
1976年 | 19篇 |
1975年 | 16篇 |
1974年 | 13篇 |
1973年 | 17篇 |
1967年 | 12篇 |
排序方式: 共有2845条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
Simon P. Liversedge Kevin B. Paterson Emma L. Clayes 《The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology》2002,55(1):225-240
We report an eye movement experiment investigating the influence of the focus operator only on syntactic processing of "long" relative clause sentences. Paterson, Liversedge, and Underwood (1999) found that readers were garden pathed by "short" reduced relative clause sentences containing the focus operator only . They argued that due to thematic differences between "short" and "long" relative clause sentences, garden path effect might not occur when "long" reduced relative clause sentences are read. Eye-tracking data show that garden path effects found during initial processing of the disambiguating verb of "long" reduced sentences without only were absent or delayed in the case of counterparts with only . We discuss our results in terms of current theories of sentence processing. 相似文献
2.
3.
Kevin B. Paterson Josephine Read Victoria A. McGowan Timothy R. Jordan 《Developmental science》2015,18(2):335-343
Developing readers often make anagrammatical errors (e.g. misreading pirates as parties), suggesting they use letter position flexibly during word recognition. However, while it is widely assumed that the occurrence of these errors decreases with increases in reading skill, empirical evidence to support this distinction is lacking. Accordingly, we compared the performance of developing child readers (aged 8–10 years) against the end‐state performance of skilled adult readers in a timed naming task, employing anagrams used previously in this area of research. Moreover, to explore the use of letter position by developing readers and skilled adult readers more fully, we used anagrams which, to form another word, required letter transpositions over only interior letter positions, or both interior and exterior letter positions. The patterns of effects across these two anagram types for the two groups of readers were very similar. In particular, both groups showed similarly slowed response times (and developing readers increased errors) for anagrams requiring only interior letter transpositions but not for anagrams that required exterior letter transpositions. This similarity in the naming performance of developing readers and skilled adult readers suggests that the end‐state skilled use of letter position is established earlier during reading development than is widely assumed. 相似文献
4.
5.
Michael S. Wogalter D. Bradley Marwitz David C. Leonard 《Applied cognitive psychology》1992,6(5):443-453
The present research examined whether line-ups based on target (‘suspect’) face similarity are biased or suggestive. Four experiments are described in which subjects constructed photographic line-ups by selecting foils similar in appearance to a target. Later, another group of subjects who had not seen the faces before (mock witnesses) were asked to pick out the targets from the line-ups. All four experiments showed that mock witnesses selected the target significantly more often than expected by chance, thereby demonstrating suggestiveness. Three alternative line-up construction methods were also evaluated. In these methods, foil selection was based not only on target similarity but also on similarity with one or more of the other line-up faces. Results showed that alternative line-up targets were not selected significantly more often than chance, suggesting that bias was reduced. An overall analysis showed that the alternative line-ups were significantly less suggestive than target-based line-ups. The results indicate that foil selection procedures that incorporate foil-to-foil similarity produce fairer line-ups than those exclusively based on target similarity. 相似文献
6.
Jonathan Grainger J. Kevin O’Regan Arthur M. Jacobs Juan Segui 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1992,51(1):49-56
Two experiments are described that measured lexical decision latencies and errors to five-letter French words with a single higher frequency orthographic neighbor and control words with no higher frequency neighbors. The higher frequency neighbor differed from the stimulus word by either the second letter (e.g.,astre-autre) or the fourth letter (chope-chose). Neighborhood frequency effects were found to interact with this factor, and significant interference was observed only tochope-type words. The effects of neighborhood frequency were also found to interact with the position of initial fixation in the stimulus word (either the second letter or the fourth letter). Interference was greatly reduced when the initial fixation was on the critical disambiguating letter (i.e., the letterp inchope). Moreover, word recognition was improved when subjects initially fixated the second letter relative to when they initially fixated the fourth letter of a five-letter word, but this second-letter advantage practically disappeared when the stimulus differed from a more frequent word by its fourth letter. The results are interpreted in terms of the interaction between visual and lexical factors in visual word recognition. 相似文献
7.
Mark D. Rapport Sheila O. Quinn George J. DuPaul Eugene P. Quinn Kevin L. Kelly 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》1989,17(6):669-689
This investigation examined the relationship between methylphenidate (MPH) and the learning and recall of paired associations by children with attention deficit disorder with hyperactivity (ADDH). Forty-five children with ADDH were randomly assigned to one of three groups (novel, partial mastery, and mastery learning) that varied in the amount of previous learning of paired associations and participated in a double-blind, placebo-control, repeated-measures-across-dose (crossover) design. Each child received four doses of MPH (5 mg, 10 mg, 15 mg, and 20 mg) and a placebo in a random, counterbalanced sequence. The results indicated that both the rate of acquisition and accuracy in learning paired associations were significantly, but differentially, affected by MPH dose and the degree of learning mastery. The implications of these results for psychopharmacological research and the monitoring of psychostimulant effects on children's learning performance in academic settings are discussed.The authors wish to acknowledge and express their sincere appreciation to the graduate and undergraduate student members of the Children's Learning Clinic. 相似文献
8.
Kevin Fauteux Ph.D. 《Journal of religion and health》1989,28(3):195-206
Artistic presentations of Jesus are as numerous and varied as the artists who created them. Whether on canvas or in music, Jesus has been portrayed as a redeemer, revolutionary, teacher, and clown. While some people are inspired by a particular presentation of Jesus, others are angered and incensed at what they perceive to be blasphemous. An example of the latter is Martin Scorsese's film, The Last Temptation of Christ. This article examines why people responded so negatively to this and other artistic presentations of Jesus. It suggests they have failed to recognize how the portrayal reflects a personal experience of the artist and is not meant to be a final portrait of Christ. And on a more unconscious level, these works of art evoke feelings within people which they fear to acknowledge and which they escape by condemning the work. 相似文献
9.
10.