全文获取类型
收费全文 | 668篇 |
免费 | 47篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
716篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 3篇 |
2023年 | 8篇 |
2022年 | 7篇 |
2021年 | 12篇 |
2020年 | 31篇 |
2019年 | 22篇 |
2018年 | 45篇 |
2017年 | 32篇 |
2016年 | 48篇 |
2015年 | 19篇 |
2014年 | 32篇 |
2013年 | 78篇 |
2012年 | 33篇 |
2011年 | 40篇 |
2010年 | 25篇 |
2009年 | 24篇 |
2008年 | 20篇 |
2007年 | 27篇 |
2006年 | 20篇 |
2005年 | 14篇 |
2004年 | 19篇 |
2003年 | 19篇 |
2002年 | 6篇 |
2001年 | 15篇 |
2000年 | 10篇 |
1999年 | 10篇 |
1998年 | 3篇 |
1997年 | 3篇 |
1995年 | 6篇 |
1994年 | 2篇 |
1992年 | 6篇 |
1990年 | 9篇 |
1989年 | 4篇 |
1988年 | 2篇 |
1987年 | 2篇 |
1986年 | 6篇 |
1985年 | 7篇 |
1984年 | 3篇 |
1983年 | 7篇 |
1982年 | 6篇 |
1981年 | 4篇 |
1980年 | 2篇 |
1979年 | 3篇 |
1977年 | 4篇 |
1976年 | 2篇 |
1975年 | 3篇 |
1972年 | 2篇 |
1969年 | 1篇 |
1967年 | 2篇 |
1966年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有716条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
2.
Vicky V. Wolfe John D. Cone David A. Wolfe 《Journal of psychopathology and behavioral assessment》1986,8(3):211-226
The need to train accurate, not necessarily agreeing, observers is discussed. Intraobserver consistency as an intermediate criterion in such training is proposed and contrasted with the more familiar criterion of interobserver agreement. Videotaped observations of social interactions between handicapped and nonhandicapped preschoolers provided the medium for examining the criterion agreement of four observers trained against each type of standard. Observers generally failed to show high levels of criterion agreement whether trained to a within- or to a between-observer agreement standard. The results varied somewhat with the frequency of behaviors, however. Correlations between interobserver agreement and intraobserver consistency were variable but somewhat higher when interobserver agreement was the training criterion than when intraobserver consistency was the criterion. Correlations between interobserver agreement and criterion agreement ranged from — .16 to .89 during interobserver agreement training. Correlations between intraobserver consistency and criterion agreement ranged from — .23 to .99 during intraobserver consistency training. 相似文献
3.
Revision of the self-monitoring scale 总被引:14,自引:0,他引:14
Snyder's (1974) Self-Monitoring Scale exhibits a stable factor structure that does not correspond to the five-component theoretical structure he presents. Sets of face-valid items that better approximate the theoretical structure are described. Correlations between these sets of items and measures of other constructs reveal that four of the five components are positively related to social anxiety. Effective social interaction is supposedly the high self-monitor's forte, and social anxiety appears to be incompatible with this. The correlational results therefore question the entire theory and indicate the need for a narrower definition of the construct. Adopting such a definition from Snyder's review article (1979), we present a 13-item Revised Self-Monitoring scale which measures only sensitivity to the expressive behavior of others and ability to modify self-presentation. A 20-item Concern for Appropriateness scale is also described. This scale measures 2 variables that are directly associated with social anxiety--cross-situational variability and attention to social comparison information. Both scales have acceptable internal consistency, and both yield 2 subscale scores as well as a total score. Prospective users of either scale are advised to treat the 3 scores separately. 相似文献
4.
Roger Ulrich Marshall Wolfe Sylvia Dulaney 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》1969,12(6):1009-1015
Squirrel monkeys were restrained in a chair equipped with a tail-shock apparatus and a pneumatic bite hose located in front of the subject's face. An aggressive response was recorded when the monkey bit the hose. Initial sessions in which no shocks were delivered produced some biting. When biting during these sessions stabilized at a near-zero level, regularly scheduled shocks were delivered to the monkey's tail, causing a consistently higher rate of biting. After several sessions under these conditions, a punishment phase was introduced in which the previous shock conditions were maintained, and every bite was followed immediately by another, more intense shock. Biting under these conditions was suppressed to a near-zero level. When the punishment contingency was removed, biting increased. With one subject, two additional bite-contingent stimuli were examined: (1) a milder shock that, when made contingent upon hose biting, also suppressed that response, and (2) a contingent tone that had no obvious suppressing or facilitating effect. Individual differences among subjects were extreme, but the effect of bite-contingent shock was consistent. Observations of the subjects during the punishment sessions indicated the existence of certain side effects that resulted from the use of punishment to suppress shock-induced aggression. 相似文献
5.
6.
Influence of spatial frequency, luminance, and duration on binocular rivalry and abnormal fusion of briefly presented dichoptic stimuli 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
J M Wolfe 《Perception》1983,12(4):447-456
Orthogonal gratings, presented dichoptically, do not fuse into a single percept. Parts of each are seen while other parts are suppressed in an unstable perception (binocular rivalry). However, it has been previously noted that, if the gratings are briefly flashed, they will appear to fuse into a plaid or checkerboard pattern. Three experiments are reported which have been designed to define more clearly the spatial and temporal parameters of this effect in the hope that this would lead to better understanding of the normal mechanisms of dominance and suppression. Stimuli appear fused if flashed for less than 150 ms. The effect is independent of substantial changes in spatial frequency and luminance. Single flashes that appear fused when presented in isolation produce rivalry if separated by intervals less than about 150 ms. Intervals greater than 150 ms produce continued abnormal fusion. Possible mechanisms are discussed. 相似文献
7.
This study investigated the effects of arranging task events for interdependence, to increase the probability of social responding. During task interdependence, the subjects, participating in dyads and a four-person group, obtained task materials (a puzzle piece) from their partner before completing their task (appropriately placing the puzzle piece). The verbal contingency required a verbal request to precede a subject's receiving a task material from his partner. The verbal contingency yoked with task interdependence made task completion contingent on the appropriate verbalization. The findings from two experiments suggested that task interdependence was sufficient to increase partner-directed verbalizations for three of the four subjects. When the verbal contingency was added, all subjects increased their requests and other verbalizations to partner. Applied to a four-person group, the verbal contingency yoked with varying levels of task interdependence correspondingly affected the pattern and level of group communications. The greater the task interdependence, i.e., the more members each subject depended on to complete his task, the more complex the social network of verbal contacts, and the higher the level of both requests and other verbalizations for the group. 相似文献
8.
Schmidt TA Delorio NM McClure KB 《The American journal of bioethics : AJOB》2006,6(3):30-2; discussion W46-8
9.
Wolfe CR 《Behavior research methods》2006,38(2):245-250
SCiP history may be divided into three eras: the Paleozoic (1971–1982), the Mesozoic (1982–1994), and the Cenozoic (1994-present).
Following a list of Secretary-Treasurers, a list of all SCiP Presidents is provided in Table 1. Next I present personal highlights,
including the first symposium on psychology and the World-Wide Web; David Rumelhart’s mathematical explanation of connectionism;
and Stevan Harnad’s discussion of “freeing” the journal literature. I observe that a small conference is becoming more intimate
and that much of our mission involves figuring out how to conduct high-quality scientific research with consumer-grade electronics.
I argue that we are an increasingly international organization, that graduate students are welcome, and that we should become
more inclusive in the areas of gender and ethnicity and should make membership more meaningful. I conclude by looking ahead
and attempting to predict the future. 相似文献
10.
The religious economies model has been influential in the sociology of religion. Yet, propositions drawn from the model have been difficult to test in the comparative and historical study of religion, generally for lack of appropriate data. We develop a general theory of religious disestablishment and apply it to the Reformation in 16th‐century Europe to explain variation in the abolition of the Catholic monopoly. We suggest three principal factors—changes in demand, entry control mechanisms, and political incentives—that explain why incumbent religious firms may lose their monopoly. We then analyze the resulting hypotheses in a systematic analysis of cities in the Holy Roman Empire. Our analysis yields mixed support for demand‐side factors and entry control mechanisms, and firm support for political incentives in the institution of reform. 相似文献