全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1388篇 |
免费 | 50篇 |
专业分类
1438篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 9篇 |
2022年 | 9篇 |
2021年 | 11篇 |
2020年 | 24篇 |
2019年 | 29篇 |
2018年 | 29篇 |
2017年 | 31篇 |
2016年 | 38篇 |
2015年 | 31篇 |
2014年 | 29篇 |
2013年 | 162篇 |
2012年 | 60篇 |
2011年 | 66篇 |
2010年 | 32篇 |
2009年 | 44篇 |
2008年 | 57篇 |
2007年 | 58篇 |
2006年 | 63篇 |
2005年 | 55篇 |
2004年 | 55篇 |
2003年 | 35篇 |
2002年 | 42篇 |
2001年 | 19篇 |
2000年 | 14篇 |
1999年 | 19篇 |
1998年 | 30篇 |
1997年 | 30篇 |
1996年 | 14篇 |
1995年 | 24篇 |
1994年 | 27篇 |
1993年 | 17篇 |
1992年 | 14篇 |
1991年 | 22篇 |
1990年 | 14篇 |
1989年 | 16篇 |
1988年 | 9篇 |
1987年 | 11篇 |
1986年 | 18篇 |
1985年 | 12篇 |
1984年 | 15篇 |
1983年 | 10篇 |
1982年 | 19篇 |
1981年 | 12篇 |
1980年 | 10篇 |
1979年 | 14篇 |
1978年 | 10篇 |
1977年 | 12篇 |
1976年 | 10篇 |
1975年 | 7篇 |
1974年 | 7篇 |
排序方式: 共有1438条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
2.
Depressive characteristics of physically abused children 总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4
Physically abused and nonabused children were compared on child-completed measures of depression, hopelessness, self-esteem, and locus of control. Results indicated that, in comparison with nonabused controls, abused children evidenced more depressive symptoms, heightened externality, lower self-esteem, and greater hopelessness about the future. Group differences in depressive symptomatology were not accounted for on the basis of differences in age, sex, race, gender, IQ, or socioeconomic status. Results replicate the results of Kazdin, Moser, Colbus, and Bell (1985) derived from a sample of physically abused psychiatric inpatients and extend the generality of these findings to abused children of nonpatient status. Implications of the findings for clinical interventions, theoretical models of child depression, and future research are discussed.The authors wish to thank Alvin Hadley and the staff of Franklin County Children Services.Appreciation is also extended to Dr. Charles Wenar. 相似文献
3.
Deficient processing theories of the spacing effect attribute poor recall of massed-repeated items to a failure to process one or both of the presentations fully. An implication of this approach is that anything that increases the probability that a repetition will receive full processing, or conversely, anything that decreases the probability that the item will be recognized as a repetition, should improve memorability of the item. The present set of experiments tested this prediction by manipulating the surface structure of repeated sentences. On the basis of previous research, it was assumed that memory for surface structure of sentences decays rapidly, and hence can contribute to initial identification of repetitions only at short spacings. Because this manipulation should hinder recognition of repetitions as repetitions, it was expected to induce full processing of massed repetitions, and thus facilitate recall of these items. This prediction was supported. When sentences were repeated verbatim (Experiment 1) or by the same speaker (Experiment 2), the typical spacing effect was obtained. However, when the surface structure or speaker changed at time of repetition, massed repetitions were recalled nearly as well (Experiment 1) or as well (Experiment 2) as their spaced counterparts. 相似文献
4.
Joseph M. Byrne Daphne J. Smith Joan E. Backman 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》1987,15(2):239-246
This study evaluated the clinical validity of the Intellectual Screening, Development, and Achievement scales of the Personality Inventory for Children-Revised (PIC-R), as well as the newly derived PIC-R Factor IV scale, in identifying cognitive impairment in a clinic-referred sample of preschoolers. The clinical validity of these PIC-R subscales was also compared to that of the Minnesota Child Development Inventory (MCDI). The results showed that the PIC-R scales and Factor IV were reasonably successful in identifying cognitively impaired preschoolers, but much less successful in identifying cognitively normal preschoolers. Overall, the MCDI was the most successful in correctly identifying the preschoolers' cognitive status. 相似文献
5.
Joan Daniels Pedro 《Journal of Vocational Behavior》1984,25(1):80-95
Some form of work experience or internship ranks high on the list of criteria used by recruiters of college graduates for positions as management trainees. While internships are discussed positively by students, faculty, and recruiters, their evaluation has been faulted as being highly subjective and lacking in the use of controls and psychometric sophistication. This study used an Institutional Cycle design to investigate changes occurring after participation in internships with three cohorts of college age students. Changes were noted in both terminal and instrumental values and in work-specific needs. In the former, changes moved the profiles of the students closer to those of campus recruiters. In the latter, changes moved the students' needs in the direction of characteristics of the job, away from environmental characteristics. 相似文献
6.
Though many studies have examined the predictive power of expressed choices versus that of measured interests, fewer have been concerned with the relationship between the degree to which choices and interests agree and later persistence in an expressed choice. This study explored that relationship, using longitudinal data for 7706 college students and 1372 employed persons. Four levels of congruence between expressed choice and measured interests were identified. Results indicated that persistence in an expressed choice increases systematically as congruence between choice and interests increases. Implications for counselors are noted. 相似文献
7.
Joan Saltor Itxaso Barberia Javier Rodríguez-Ferreiro 《Applied cognitive psychology》2023,37(2):360-368
Acceptance of fake news is probably modulated by an intricate interplay of social, cultural, and political factors. In this study, we investigated whether individual-level cognitive factors related to thinking and decision making could influence the tendency to accept fake news. A group of volunteers responded to a COVID19-related fake news discrimination scale as well as to questionnaires assessing their thinking style (reflective vs. intuitive) and thinking disposition (actively open-mindedness). Furthermore, they completed a computerized contingency learning task aimed at measuring their tendency to develop a causal illusion, a cognitive bias leading to perceive causal connections between non-contingent events. More actively open-minded and more reflective individuals presented higher fake news discrimination scores. In addition, those who developed weaker causal illusions in the contingency learning task were also more accurate at differentiating between fake and legitimate news. Actively open-minded thinking was the main contributor in a regression model predicting fake news discrimination. 相似文献
8.
The relative efficiency of five techniques of illusion measurement were tested on parametric variations of the Mueller-Lyer and Ebbinghaus figures. The methods of average error, reproduction, and selection from a graded series all showed significant effects of configurational variations. The subjective scaling techniques each failed to measure adequately changes in illusion magnitude for one illusion configuration. The suitability of the tested measures for group administration is also discussed. 相似文献
9.
Joan Z. Rosenblith 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》1970,14(2):139-144
Drinking was studied in rats pressing a bar on a second-order schedule in which every third completion of a 1-min fixed interval was followed by food presentation. A brief flash of light signaled the completion of each fixed-interval component. The rats drank not only after the food presentations but also after presentations of the light flash alone. A high rate of steady drinking followed intervals terminated by a food presentation. Drinking that followed intervals terminated by a light flash alone was of comparable rate, but characteristically interrupted by bar pressing. When 250-mg food pellets were used instead of 45-mg pellets, both drinking and bar-pressing rates increased substantially. 相似文献
10.
Of 23 pigeons, 11 received continuous reinforcement for key pecking, and 12 received an FR 10 schedule of reinforcement. The birds were then tested without food, but with potential conditioned reinforcers presented either on the same schedule as in training, on the other schedule, or not at all. Each bird in the subgroup trained on CRF and tested with Sr's at FR 10 not only gave more responses in testing than did each bird in both subgroups receiving no Sr's, but also gave more responses than did each bird in the Sr subgroup receiving CRF training and Sr's at CRF. Cumulative records are presented to show the effects of different schedules of conditioned reinforcers. 相似文献