全文获取类型
收费全文 | 179篇 |
免费 | 7篇 |
专业分类
186篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 2篇 |
2022年 | 3篇 |
2021年 | 3篇 |
2020年 | 4篇 |
2019年 | 3篇 |
2018年 | 10篇 |
2017年 | 2篇 |
2016年 | 10篇 |
2015年 | 4篇 |
2014年 | 14篇 |
2013年 | 41篇 |
2012年 | 10篇 |
2011年 | 9篇 |
2010年 | 8篇 |
2009年 | 10篇 |
2008年 | 6篇 |
2007年 | 10篇 |
2006年 | 5篇 |
2005年 | 5篇 |
2004年 | 3篇 |
2003年 | 4篇 |
2001年 | 4篇 |
2000年 | 4篇 |
1999年 | 4篇 |
1996年 | 1篇 |
1989年 | 1篇 |
1987年 | 1篇 |
1981年 | 1篇 |
1979年 | 2篇 |
1978年 | 1篇 |
1976年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有186条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
Henning Gibbons Thomas H. Rammsayer 《The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology》2004,57(1):61-86
A series of four spatial localization experiments is reported that examined the effects of display duration and presentation mode on positive and negative priming using an attended-repetition and an ignored-repetition paradigm, respectively. Experiment 1 showed larger positive priming with response-dependent than with 150 ms display durations while negative priming remained unaffected. Experiments 2-4 were performed to further elucidate the effects of prime-probe durations. Data suggest largely independent effects of prime and probe duration on priming effects. Manipulation of prime duration affected facilitation due to repetition of the prime distractor location as well as inhibitory effects associated with ignored repetition. Furthermore, anticipated probe duration modulated the effectiveness of inhibition of return. Findings are discussed within a framework proposing two major components of priming effects—a stimulus-driven or automatic component, and a strategic component related to the participant's expectations towards the probe. 相似文献
2.
A L Combs D A Beezley G M Prater G F Henning R F Cottrell 《Perceptual and motor skills》1979,49(3):867-870
Among a group of 12 persons selected for the ability to write with ease with either hand, none were found to write using a hooked hand posture with either the right or left hand. Tests of verbal and manipulospatial ability indicated a normal balance of these two types of abilities, usually associated with the left and right hemispheres. Findings are discussed in terms of implications for cerebral organization and hand position in writing. 相似文献
3.
4.
Schmidgen H 《History of psychology》2005,8(1):46-78
In Wilhelm Wundt's (1832-1920) Leipzig laboratory and at numerous other research sites, the chronoscope was used to conduct reaction time experiments. The author argues that the history of the chronoscope is the history not of an instrument but of an experimental setup. This setup was initially devised by the English physicist and instrument maker Charles Wheatstone (1802-1875) in the early 1840s. Shortly thereafter, it was improved by the German clockmaker and mechanic Matth?us Hipp (1813-1893). In the 1850s, the chronoscope was introduced to ballistic research. In the early 1860s, Neuchatel astronomer Adolphe Hirsch (1830-1901) applied it to the problem of physiological time. The extensions and variations of chronoscope use within the contexts of ballistics, physiology, and psychology presented special challenges. These challenges were met with specific attempts to reduce the errors in chronoscopic experiments on shooting stands and in the psychological laboratory. 相似文献
5.
Infants' sensitivity to social contingencies was assessed. In Study 1, 1-month-old infants and their mothers interacted face-to-face in three types of imperfect contingent interactions: Normal, Non-Contingent and Imitation. One-month-old infants did not discriminate these conditions. In Study 2, 3-month-old infants were tested as in Study 1. At 3 months of age, infants gazed reliably longer in the Imitation condition and smiled reliably more in the Normal than in the Non-Contingent and Imitation interactions. These findings suggest a developmental transition in the sensitivity to social contingencies between 1 and 3 months of age. The relationship between the developing sensitivity to social contingencies and social cognition is discussed. 相似文献
6.
Henning Saß 《Forensische Psychiatrie, Psychologie, Kriminologie》2010,4(4):223-232
Querulous behavior and querulous paranoia are seldom in forensic settings, but produce major problems because of the chronicity and severity of the disorders. Aspects of psychopathology and differential diagnosis are discussed, especially characteristics of volition, of cognition and of behavior control. Regarding criminal responsibility the capacity of understanding the wrongfulness of behavior will usually not be severely impaired. In cases of strong deformation of personality structure with transition into paranoid disorder the capacity for behavior control may be significantly diminished. Irresponsibility will only occur in the rare cases of psychotic disorders. Because of a poor prognosis detention in forensic hospitals may be necessary when threats and manifest aggressive behavior occur. 相似文献
7.
8.
9.
Dr. phil. Johannes C. Ehrenthal Dipl.-Psych. Julia Tomanek Henning Schauenburg Ulrike Dinger 《Psychotherapeut》2013,58(5):474-479
Background
The therapeutic relationship is a central factor for psychotherapy, which may be at least partially explained by attachment theory. However, research concerning the conditions under which attachment-related needs are activated in treatment followed by possibly dysfunctional coping strategies is rare.Patients and methods
Semi-structured interviews on attachment-related situations in psychotherapies were conducted with 36 inpatients and then rated with regard to content.Results
Triggers for attachment-related needs in therapies were interpersonal conflict and loss, beginning and end of therapy, symptom burden, specific interventions, addressing the therapeutic relationship and change of setting from outpatient to inpatient treatment. This was related to themes of desire for help and support, experience of rejection, self-disclosure and desire for understanding.Conclusions
The attachment system is activated in therapies similar to other interpersonal situations. The results may help to become more sensitive to attachment-related situations and needs. 相似文献10.
The present study aimed at investigating whether different aspects of performance on a prospective reproduction task with a standard duration of 2 s had different antecedents in the event-related potentials (ERPs) accompanying standard presentations and reproduced intervals. On each trial, first the standard duration was presented as an empty auditory interval. After a short delay, participants reproduced this interval by means of two button presses defining onset and offset of the reproduced interval. About 25 participants were divided into groups of poor and good time estimators, once based on the coefficient of variation of their reproduced durations, and once based on the absolute error score. Interestingly, for both performance measures differences between participants were accompanied by ERP differences during standard presentations rather than reproduction itself. While larger P300 amplitudes evoked by standard interval offset predicted good reproduction performance in terms of a small coefficient of variation, small absolute error scores were accompanied by larger fronto-central negative slow wave during standard presentations. The results suggest an important role of attention to both the critical events that delimit the to-be-estimated temporal intervals, and to the passing of time between these events. 相似文献