全文获取类型
收费全文 | 333篇 |
免费 | 10篇 |
专业分类
343篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 4篇 |
2021年 | 8篇 |
2020年 | 9篇 |
2019年 | 5篇 |
2018年 | 7篇 |
2017年 | 10篇 |
2016年 | 16篇 |
2015年 | 8篇 |
2014年 | 12篇 |
2013年 | 32篇 |
2012年 | 14篇 |
2011年 | 17篇 |
2010年 | 11篇 |
2009年 | 9篇 |
2008年 | 18篇 |
2007年 | 19篇 |
2006年 | 10篇 |
2005年 | 16篇 |
2004年 | 8篇 |
2003年 | 7篇 |
2002年 | 7篇 |
2001年 | 6篇 |
1999年 | 3篇 |
1998年 | 5篇 |
1997年 | 3篇 |
1995年 | 6篇 |
1992年 | 3篇 |
1989年 | 4篇 |
1987年 | 2篇 |
1984年 | 2篇 |
1983年 | 4篇 |
1982年 | 3篇 |
1981年 | 1篇 |
1980年 | 2篇 |
1978年 | 4篇 |
1977年 | 7篇 |
1976年 | 3篇 |
1975年 | 3篇 |
1974年 | 1篇 |
1973年 | 4篇 |
1971年 | 6篇 |
1968年 | 3篇 |
1967年 | 3篇 |
1966年 | 1篇 |
1965年 | 1篇 |
1962年 | 2篇 |
1961年 | 1篇 |
1960年 | 1篇 |
1958年 | 2篇 |
1957年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有343条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
2.
3.
M J Strube C L Lott R Heilizer B Gregg 《Journal of personality and social psychology》1986,50(2):403-412
We investigated the control judgments of Type A and B actors and observers after five tasks in which actual response-outcome contingency and success were varied systematically. Results indicated that, overall, actors provided higher control judgments than did observers, and both actual contingency and success influenced judged control. Type A and B actors did not differ in their self-perceptions of control but observers judged the Type A actors to have exerted more control than the Type B actors, primarily on positive contingency tasks. These findings suggest that Type As, because of their more active, dynamic style, may be credited by observers with more control or competence than is warranted. By contrast, the more relaxed style of the Type B may lead to lower than warranted evaluations of control or competence. Lastly, Type As were found to learn the contingencies better than Type Bs with important implications for the actual exercise of control. 相似文献
4.
5.
Gregg C. Oden 《Memory & cognition》1978,6(1):26-37
It is proposed that the degree of sensibleness of sentences is determined by semantic constraints which may be more or less satisfied. Such continuous semantic constraints were examined in two experiments in which subjects judged the likelihood of obtaining each of the interpretations of ambiguous sentences. The sentences were factorially generated by independently varying the degree to which semantic constraints for each interpretation were satisfied. In one experiment, the semantic constraints were manipulated by varying critical words within the ambiguous sentence; in the other experiment, a preceding context sentence was used. The results of both experiments supported the hypotheses that the judged likelihood was a direct function of the relative sensibleness of the interpretations, that semantic constraints determined the degree of sensibleness of each interpretation, and that these semantic constraints are continuous restrictions which are independent of each other and stable from sentence to sentence in which they occur. 相似文献
6.
Gregg Andrew Hurwitz 《Sexuality & culture》2002,6(3):3-73
This essay applies a Freudian psychoanalytic and Jungian archetypal narrative analysis to Shakespeare’s first and oft-criticized
romance. The author argues that key structural and thematic elements ofPericles are best illuminated when viewed through a psychological interpretative lens, and that the play is best comprehended when
examined in the context of its associative, rather than linear, richness. Masculinity and femininity, central themes of the
narrative, are explored both in relation to the Oedipal complex and psychological individuation.Pericles also provides an excellent basis to examine key differences between Freud’s and Jung’s approach, particularly Jung’s widening
of the primarily sexual psychoanalytic approach to encompass broader archetypal meaning. 相似文献
7.
Pictorial stimuli are more likely to be recognized if they are the same size, rather than a different size, at study and at test. This size congruency effect was replicated in two experiments in which the encoding variables were respectively undivided versus divided attention and level of processing. In terms of performance, these variables influenced recognition and did not influence size congruency effects. But in terms of awareness, measured by remember and know responses, these variables did influence size congruency effects. With undivided attention and with a deep level of processing, size congruency effects occurred only in remembering. With divided attention and with a shallow level of processing, size congruency effects occurred only in knowing. The results show that effects that occur in remembering may also occur independently in knowing. They support theories in which remembering and knowing reflect different memory processes or systems. They do not support the theory that remembering and knowing reflect differences in trace strength. 相似文献
8.
In the simulation of human behavior on a digital computer, one first attempts to discover the manner in which subjects (Ss) internally represent the environment and the rules that they employ for acting upon this representation. The interaction between the rules and the environmental representation over a period of time constitutes a set of processes. Processes can be expressed as flow charts which, in turn, are stated formally in terms of a computer program. The program serves as a theory which is tested by executing the program on a computer and comparing the machine's performance with S's behavior. 相似文献
9.
Synthese - Veritism, the position that truth is necessary for epistemic acceptability, seems to be in tension with the observation that much of our best science is not, strictly speaking, true when... 相似文献
10.
Léa Entzmann Nathalie Guyader Louise Kauffmann Juliette Lenouvel Clémence Charles Carole Peyrin Roman Vuillaume Martial Mermillod 《Cognitive Science》2021,45(10):e13042
Previous studies have shown that the human visual system can detect a face and elicit a saccadic eye movement toward it very efficiently compared to other categories of visual stimuli. In the first experiment, we tested the influence of facial expressions on fast face detection using a saccadic choice task. Face-vehicle pairs were simultaneously presented and participants were asked to saccade toward the target (the face or the vehicle). We observed that saccades toward faces were initiated faster, and more often in the correct direction, than saccades toward vehicles, regardless of the facial expressions (happy, fearful, or neutral). We also observed that saccade endpoints on face images were lower when the face was happy and higher when it was neutral. In the second experiment, we explicitly tested the detection of facial expressions. We used a saccadic choice task with emotional-neutral pairs of faces and participants were asked to saccade toward the emotional (happy or fearful) or the neutral face. Participants were faster when they were asked to saccade toward the emotional face. They also made fewer errors, especially when the emotional face was happy. Using computational modeling, we showed that this happy face advantage can, at least partly, be explained by perceptual factors. Also, saccade endpoints were lower when the target was happy than when it was fearful. Overall, we suggest that there is no automatic prioritization of emotional faces, at least for saccades with short latencies, but that salient local face features can automatically attract attention. 相似文献