全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1493篇 |
免费 | 53篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
1548篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 18篇 |
2019年 | 36篇 |
2018年 | 25篇 |
2017年 | 34篇 |
2016年 | 32篇 |
2015年 | 27篇 |
2014年 | 39篇 |
2013年 | 163篇 |
2012年 | 51篇 |
2011年 | 57篇 |
2010年 | 49篇 |
2009年 | 35篇 |
2008年 | 49篇 |
2007年 | 53篇 |
2006年 | 50篇 |
2005年 | 51篇 |
2004年 | 43篇 |
2003年 | 40篇 |
2002年 | 50篇 |
2001年 | 29篇 |
2000年 | 28篇 |
1999年 | 25篇 |
1998年 | 15篇 |
1997年 | 20篇 |
1996年 | 16篇 |
1995年 | 26篇 |
1994年 | 13篇 |
1993年 | 18篇 |
1992年 | 17篇 |
1991年 | 15篇 |
1990年 | 15篇 |
1989年 | 14篇 |
1988年 | 19篇 |
1987年 | 18篇 |
1986年 | 12篇 |
1985年 | 22篇 |
1984年 | 21篇 |
1983年 | 14篇 |
1981年 | 12篇 |
1980年 | 15篇 |
1979年 | 19篇 |
1978年 | 13篇 |
1977年 | 18篇 |
1976年 | 19篇 |
1975年 | 18篇 |
1974年 | 19篇 |
1973年 | 15篇 |
1971年 | 14篇 |
1970年 | 14篇 |
1967年 | 17篇 |
排序方式: 共有1548条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
2.
This study examined the agreement or congruence rate between clinical-discharge diagnoses rendered by a psychiatrist, and admission and discharge MMPI-derived diagnoses from four diagnostic classification systems that have been developed for the MMPI. The four classification systems included a simple high-point code based on the most elevated clinical scale in the profile, the Henrichs revision of the Meehl-Dahlstrom rules, the Goldberg equations, and a system developed by Lachar. Subjects consisted of 150 patients selected from a larger pool of patients who had completed a 9-week adult residential treatment program. Overall, this study yielded modest hit rates between 26% and 34% for MMPI-derived diagnoses and psychiatric diagnoses across the various classification systems. In addition, stability of MMPI-based diagnoses from admission to discharge assessments ranged from 48% to 51% depending on the classification system employed. Findings are discussed in terms of their implications for the use of the MMPI in patient diagnosis. It is recommended that the MMPI be used in conjunction with other sources of clinical and test information in deriving clinical diagnoses. 相似文献
3.
Gordon W. Russell Sherry L. Di Lullo Dany Di Lullo 《Current psychology (New Brunswick, N.J.)》1988,7(4):312-321
Males were randomly assigned to view either (1) a film clip featuring hockey fights or (2) a film of nonaggressive hockey
action or (3) a no-film control condition after having first been angered or treated politely by an experimental confederate.
The dependent variable was represented by a measure of aggressive mood and a behavioral measure of retaliatory aggression.
Analyses revealed that both angered and nonangered subjects exhibited an increase in aggressive mood following exposure to
the fight film. However, the analysis involving retaliatory aggression against the confederate yielded an anger x film interaction.
While angered subjects were more aggressive than nonangered, only angered subjects retaliated against the confederate after
viewing the fight film. The results were discussed in terms of Berkowitz’s (1974) aggressive cue theory.
A version of this paper was presented at the meeting of the North American Society for the Sociology of Sport, Edmonton, Canada,
November 1987. 相似文献
4.
Gordon Halliday Ph.D. 《Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy》1992,22(3):173-181
The concept of developmental amnesia is proposed to describe the impairment of normal memories during a person's school age life. One hundred psychotherapy patients were interviewed and the age of earliest memories ranged from 1–12 years, with a mode of 5 years. Memory was continuous for 53% of patients by 1st grade, while 18% reported life memory was still episodic at age 18 or later. Parental alcohol problems were associated with delay of continuous memories. 相似文献
5.
Explanations of context effects in the Reicher-Wheeler task and the letter-identification task appeal to word-based processing, yet these tasks provide no explicit measure of word processing. An experiment is reported which was designed to investigate the use of transfer in the word-identification task as a measure of word-based processing in letter-identification tasks. It was found that encoding manipulations that determined whether a word-superiority effect was or was not found in a letter-identification task (e.g., Thompson & Massaro, 1973) also determined whether transfer was or was not found in a subsequent word-identification task. The results of the experiment are discussed in terms of the utility of using transfer experiments as converging evidence about the presence and/or absence of processes that cannot be directly measured in other experimental paradigms. 相似文献
6.
Rats were given backward pairings of a tone (CS) and shock (US) and were then tested for response to the CS in a lick suppression test. Animals given a cuing or reminder treatment prior to the test exhibited enhanced suppression in the presence of the tone relative to a variety of control conditions. The cue-induced suppression enhancement did not appear to result from sensitization or short-term motivational changes nor did it seem to depend on second-order conditioning of the test context. The effect appeared more robust as the number of backward conditioning trials preceding the cue increased. The results were discussed in terms of current explanations of backward conditioning effects. 相似文献
7.
Gordon L. Flett Karel Vredenburg Patricia Pliner Lester Krames 《Journal of research in personality》1985,19(4):429-435
The present study examined the direction of the relation between sex role self-concept and depression. A total of 23 males and 32 females were given the Personal Attributes Questionnaire and the Beck Depression Inventory at two times separated by a 3-month interval. Results of a cross-lagged panel correlational analysis found no evidence for sex role orientation as a cause of depression. Instead, the data suggested that emotional well being precedes self-perceived instrumentality, but the influence of third variables could not be overlooked. The implications of these data are discussed with particular reference to the need for more complex theoretical formulations of the relation between self-perceived instrumentality and adjustment. 相似文献
8.
Brief presentations of letter arrays were probed by bar markers within or outside the fovea after various time delays. Figural
information presented to the foveal region was retained better under a photopic condition than information in the parafoveal
region, but under a scotopic condition parafoveal information demonstrated superior retention. However, decay rates over time
of all functions were similar, demonstrating that figural information in the rods and cones decays at similar rates. 相似文献
9.
Gordon E. O'brien 《Australian journal of psychology》1984,36(1):57-74
The purpose of this study was to examine the reciprocal relationships between locus of control and job attributes of a representative sample of 1383 employees in a large Australian city. It was predicted that employees with an internal locus of control would attain jobs higher on skill-utilization, influence and income than those attained by employees with an external locus of control. It was also predicted that jobs involving high skill-utlization, influence and income would induce greater internal control beliefs than those low on skill-utilization, influence and income. Locus of control was measured using Rotter's scale (Rotter, 1966). Reciprocal causation was assessed using two-stage least squares analysis. For the total sample, it was found that locus of control and job attributes were reciprocally determined. However, locus of control was not related to influence when skill-utilization and income were controlled. When the sample was split on the basis of gender and marital status, the predicted relationships between locus of control, skill-utilization and income were found for married men only. For married women, locus of control was neither a determinant of job attributes nor an outcome of job experience. These sub-group differences were interpreted in terms of differences in job tenure, work values and occupational opportunity. 相似文献
10.
Performance of disturbed hyperactive and nonhyperactive children on an objective measure of hyperactivity 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
Twenty hyperactive emotionally disturbed children (6–11 years) and a matched sample of nonhyperactive emotionally disturbed children were selected from the population of a therapeutic day treatment facility on the basis of teacher ratings. They were administered the Matching Familiar Figures Test-20 and were rated on several scales of impulsivity and/or hyperactivity. Each subject was required to perform on the Delay Task of the Gordon Diagnostic System, which required them to inhibit behavioral responding on a temporally based schedule (DRL-6) in order to win points. Children classified as hyperactive, whether by one or more criteria, were relatively unable to refrain from emitting a high number of nonreinforced responses. Moreover, these performance differences persisted regardless of age or IQ and were stable over the 8 minutes required to complete the test.The authors gratefully acknowledge the efforts of the following members of the Institute of Clinical Psychology, University of Virginia: Richard Abidin, Ann Loper, Ronald Reeve, and Sherry Kraft. Data analyses were conducted using STATMANAGER, Hayden Software Company, copyright 1983. 相似文献