全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1777篇 |
免费 | 29篇 |
专业分类
1806篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 14篇 |
2023年 | 10篇 |
2022年 | 17篇 |
2021年 | 22篇 |
2020年 | 40篇 |
2019年 | 52篇 |
2018年 | 62篇 |
2017年 | 59篇 |
2016年 | 77篇 |
2015年 | 53篇 |
2014年 | 40篇 |
2013年 | 223篇 |
2012年 | 107篇 |
2011年 | 81篇 |
2010年 | 52篇 |
2009年 | 68篇 |
2008年 | 90篇 |
2007年 | 88篇 |
2006年 | 62篇 |
2005年 | 65篇 |
2004年 | 62篇 |
2003年 | 55篇 |
2002年 | 50篇 |
2001年 | 24篇 |
2000年 | 25篇 |
1999年 | 23篇 |
1998年 | 19篇 |
1997年 | 19篇 |
1996年 | 16篇 |
1995年 | 17篇 |
1994年 | 11篇 |
1993年 | 19篇 |
1992年 | 16篇 |
1991年 | 11篇 |
1990年 | 9篇 |
1989年 | 14篇 |
1988年 | 10篇 |
1987年 | 9篇 |
1986年 | 9篇 |
1985年 | 15篇 |
1984年 | 11篇 |
1983年 | 10篇 |
1982年 | 7篇 |
1981年 | 5篇 |
1980年 | 7篇 |
1978年 | 5篇 |
1977年 | 7篇 |
1975年 | 9篇 |
1971年 | 5篇 |
1970年 | 5篇 |
排序方式: 共有1806条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
Summary Simple motor responses of different similarity were assigned to geometric figures of different similarity. Subsequently, subjects had to learn hierarchical classifications of visually similar or dissimilar figures which were associated with either similar or dissimilar responses. In this way, the visual and the functional similarity of the conceptually united figures were orthogonally varied. It was found that visually similar figures are conceptually classified fastest regardless of their functional similarity. In contrast, functional similarity has a considerable impact on the hierarchical classification of visually dissimilar figures. Functionally similar figures are conceptually faster classified than dissimilar ones. From the results we conclude that concepts are autonomously formed as mediating representations in stimulus-response coordinations. The hierarchical organization of concepts is thereby based on the sequential organization of both the visual input and the behavioral output. Further consequences of this approach are discussed.Portions of this research were presented at the International Symposium on Structure and Function of Human Memory, in Memoriam Hermann Ebbinghaus, Berlin, July 1985 相似文献
2.
An experiment was conducted aimed first at establishing the reliability and validity of a measure of individual differences in sleeping schedules. Two samples of subjects completed a questionnaire concerning their sleeping behavior which was scored according to the diurnal or nocturnal pattern of their responses. The scores were then used to classify subjects as morning people or evening people. Both the internal reliability and cross-time reliability of the scale were assessed and found to be high. In addition, three indices of construct validity, obtained 4 months after the administration of the scale, supported its validity. In a second part of the experiment, subjects classified as either morning or evening people rated themselves on a set of behavioral and personality dimensions. Another sample of subjects indicated their perceptions of morning people and evening people in general, along the same dimensions. It was found that subjects did have strong beliefs about personality and behavioral differences between morning people and evening people. However, the self-perceptions of the actual morning and evening people did not differ, thereby casting doubt on the validity of popular stereotypic perceptions about the two groups. 相似文献
3.
4.
5.
A PROSPECTIVE ANALYSIS OF THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG SEXUAL ASSAULT EXPERIENCES An Extension of Previous Findings 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
Christine A. Gidycz Kimberly Hanson Melissa J. Layman 《Psychology of women quarterly》1995,19(1):5-29
Research has indicated that sexual assault on college campuses is pervasive (Koss, Gidycz, & Wisniewski, 1987) and that a history of sexual victimization serves as a risk factor for future victimization (Gidycz, Coble, Latham, & Layman, 1993). The purpose of the present investigation was to extend the findings of Gidycz et al. (1993) in examining the link among sexual victimization experiences. College women were evaluated for child and adolescent sexual victimization, family adjustment, alcohol use, psychological adjustment, interpersonal functioning, and sexual behavior. Women were reevaluated at 3, 5–6, and 9 months for adult victimization, psychological adjustment, interpersonal functioning, and sexual behavior. Loglinear analysis indicated that chances of being victimized in one time period increased with greater severity of victimization in the preceding time period. The path analysis assessing the mediating effects of these variables on victimization experiences was partially supported. Implications for future research and preventative efforts are discussed. 相似文献
6.
The dominant methodological approach in psychological research has involved the use of quantitative methods within a positivist framework. In this article we argue that both qualitative and quantitative methods have their strengths and limitations, depending on the research question under investigation. We examine some of the advantages of qualitative methods, paying particular attention to the value of such methods for feminist researchers. We challenge the positivist assumption that all research should be apolitical and value-free, arguing that the political context in which all research studies take place plays an important role in decisions about the appropriate research methods to use. Despite the value attached to qualitative methods by feminist researchers, there may be projects for which quantitative methods, or a combination of qualitative and quantitative techniques, are more suitable. We draw on examples from our research on the transition from school to the job market for young people, and a study of 16- to 19-year-old first time mothers to illustrate these points, examining the practical implications of our arguments for applied social psychology research. 相似文献
7.
8.
9.
10.
Christine D. Isaacs Lynne H. Embry Donald M. Baer 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》1982,15(4):505-520
This study implemented and evaluated a training program (a written manual, videotaped models, rehearsal, role plays, and performance feedback) designed to teach five subjects the skills to become effective family therapists. The study examined the therapists' use of three target behaviors: instructing, informing, and praising. The therapists, each paired with a parent and a preschool-aged child (2 1/2–4 1/2 yr old), were trained in the clinic to use, and to teach to the parents, several behavioral skills (e.g., praising, planned ignoring, and time-out) relevant to teaching children compliance to parental instructions. A multiple-baseline design across triads (therapist/parent/child) demonstrated that after the training program was instituted, the therapists increased their rates of instructing, praising, and informing the parents; all parents increased attention to compliance, decreased attention to noncompliance, and increased rates of praise to their children; and all children increased their compliance and decreased their noncompliance. 相似文献