共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
This research was designed to examine how exposure to information about a man described in terms of gender-typed, cross gender-typed, or androgynous characteristics affected judgments about his potential satisfaction and suitability for male- and female-dominated occupations. In addition, we examined how this exposure affected judgments about another man who was applying for the same job. Drawing from prior theory and research on the schema maintenance through compensation model (e.g., Seta & Seta, 1993; Seta, Seta, & McElroy, 2003), it was predicted and found that participants with strong gender stereotypes develop compensatory expectancies. Specifically, in comparison to control conditions, participants exposed to information about the first applicant that was inconsistent with a typical man’s behavior expected the second applicant to be especially “macho” and to be unsuitable in and dissatisfied with traditionally, female-dominant occupations. Implications for employment interviews were discussed. 相似文献
2.
Competition among scientists for funding, positions and prestige, among other things, is often seen as a salutary driving
force in U.S. science. Its effects on scientists, their work and their relationships are seldom considered. Focus-group discussions
with 51 mid- and early-career scientists, on which this study is based, reveal a dark side of competition in science. According
to these scientists, competition contributes to strategic game-playing in science, a decline in free and open sharing of information
and methods, sabotage of others’ ability to use one’s work, interference with peer-review processes, deformation of relationships,
and careless or questionable research conduct. When competition is pervasive, such effects may jeopardize the progress, efficiency
and integrity of science. 相似文献
3.
Philip Schwadel 《Review of religious research》2011,53(2):161-182
I challenge the scholarly contention that increases in education uniformly lead to declines in religious participation, belief,
and affiliation. I argue that education influences strategies of action, and these strategies of action are relevant to some
religious beliefs and activities but not others. Analysis of survey data shows that (1) education negatively affects exclusivist
religious viewpoints and biblical literalism but not belief in God or the afterlife; (2) education positively affects religious
participation, devotional activities, and emphasizing the importance of religion in daily life; (3) education positively affects
switching religious affiliations, particularly to a mainline Protestant denomination, but not disaffiliation; (4) education
is positively associated with questioning the role of religion in secular society but not with support for curbing the public
opinions of religious leaders; and (5) the effects of education on religious beliefs and participation vary across religious
traditions. Education does influence Americans’ religious beliefs and activities, but the effects of education on religion
are complex. 相似文献
4.
We explored how competition affects the quality of musical improvisation, as well as the intrinsic motivation and stress reported by improvisers. Amateur musicians improvised on a keyboard in one of two conditions: induced competition and no competition. Employing the consensual assessment technique, improvisations were assessed for creativity and technical goodness by 10 expert judges. Findings indicate that improvisations were judged as more creative under competitive than non-competitive conditions. Moreover, improvisers in the competition condition were more intrinsically motivated, as well as more stressed, than improvisers in the no competition condition. The creativity and technical goodness dimensions of improvisations were positively related to each other. The findings are discussed in light of the intense debate over the effects of extrinsic motivators on intrinsic motivation and creativity and offer mechanisms through which competition may affect creative performance as well as discuss the role of stress in affecting motivation and creativity. 相似文献
5.
Duarte Angela M. Eikeseth Svein Rosales-Ruiz Jesus Baer Donald M. 《The Psychological record》1998,48(4):631-646
The Psychological Record - Two interlocking conditional discriminations were established in a paper-and-pencil match-to-sample format by written rule-like instructions to 104 college... 相似文献
6.
Jane E. Gillham Phil Putter V. Megan Kash 《Journal of psychopathology and behavioral assessment》2007,29(2):125-133
Two studies examined the hypothesis that recollections of parents’ child-rearing behaviors on the Parental Bonding Instrument
(PBI: Parker, Tupling, & Brown, 1979) are influenced by current mood. Study 1 investigated the effects of sad versus neutral
mood induction on PBI scores in 50 college students. Participants rated their parents as less caring following the sad mood
induction than following the neutral induction. Study 2 investigated the effects of naturally occurring changes in depressive
symptoms on PBI scores over a two month period. Changes in depressive symptoms correlated with changes in father care scores
and tended to correlate with changes in mother care scores. Findings from both studies suggest that retrospective reports
of parenting are susceptible to mood bias and highlight the limits of using cross sectional designs to explore the relationship
between parenting and depression. 相似文献
7.
This study examined the effects of marital conflict on Korean children’s psychological adjustment and appraisal of hypothetical
marital conflict situations. Children between the ages of 10 and 12 were divided into “high-conflict” (n = 58) and “low-conflict” (n = 58) groups based on their self-reported degree of perceived interparental conflict in the home environment. Hypothetical
marital conflict situations were provided in cartoon format, and were differentiated based conflict intensity (verbal versus
physical aggression) and content (child-related conflict versus non-child-related conflict). In general, children reported
greater negative affect and perceived threat to hypothetical conflict situations involving physical aggression compared to
situations involving verbal conflict. In child-related conflict situations, children reported more fear of being drawn in
and endorsed coping strategies that involved direct intervention. “High-conflict group” children evidenced stronger reactivity
in responding to marital conflict situations in general and endorsed indirect intervention strategies—a finding previously
not found in similar studies conducted with European-American children—indicating the possibility of cross-cultural difference
in coping preferences in interparental conflict situations. Furthermore, “high-conflict group” children manifested more indices
of maladjustment as indicated by externalizing and internalizing behavior problems, in addition to more self-reported depressive
symptoms. Results highlight the effects of marital conflict on children’s psychological adjustment and indicate the possibility
of cross-cultural differences in preferred coping mechanism in interparental conflict situations for Korean children. 相似文献
8.
Parenting behaviors have received ample support as a mediator of the relationship between maternal affect and child behavior
problems. The majority of these research efforts were based on a uni-dimensional conceptualization of maternal mood, even
though decades of theory and research suggest that mood is multidimensional. We examined the mediating role of parenting behaviors
on positive affect (PA) and negative affect (NA) and reports of child behavior problems. Evidence for mediation was demonstrated
for both PA and NA for children in early to middle childhood. Consistent with the positive psychology movement, our results
suggest that maternal PA plays an important role in parents’ utilization of effective parenting behaviors. Implications for
clinical intervention and future research are discussed. 相似文献
9.
Deanna M. Macris 《Journal of cognition and development》2017,18(3):358-374
Three experiments examined whether 4- and 5-year-olds can explicitly revise uncertain beliefs in light of disconfirming evidence. We considered 2 factors that might influence belief revision: a) the type and variability of evidence provided, and b) whether children generated an explanation of their initial hypothesis. When provided with limited observed evidence, children revised their belief about their initial guess greater than chance expectations, but differences were not observed when the quality of the explanation was considered (Experiment 1). In a similar paradigm, children revised their beliefs more often than chance when asked to explain or describe their initial belief when shown more diverse counterevidence (Experiment 2) or when told that their initial belief was incorrect (Experiment 3). Overall, these data suggest that preschoolers have nascent capacities for explicit belief revision in light of counterevidence. 相似文献
10.
Children’s attentional state during parent-child interactions is important for word learning. The current study examines the real-time attentional patterns of toddlers with and without hearing loss (N = 15, age range: 12–37 months) in parent-child interactions. High-density gaze data recorded from head-mounted eye-trackers were used to investigate the synchrony between parents’ naming of novel objects and children’s sustained attention on the named objects in joint play. Results show that the sheer quantities of parents’ naming and children’s sustained attention episodes were comparable in children with hearing loss and their peers with normal hearing. However, parents’ naming and children’s sustained attention episodes were less synchronized in the hearing loss group compared to children with normal hearing. Possible implications are discussed. 相似文献
11.
The effects of irrelevant information (0, 1, 2, or 3 bits stimulus) on absolute judgments of size were measured at different levels of discrimination difficulty. The stimuli were 7 small circular spots of light of different sizes, selected from an eaual-discriminability scole, and arranged into 6 pairs that represented different levels of stimulus dissimilarity. Three of the pairs comprised a low-dissimilarity (LD) subgrouping in which different lepels of dissimilarity and discriminability were confounded, whereas the remaining three pairs comprised a high-dissimilarity (HD) subgrouping in which different levels of dissimilarity occurred at a single high level of discriminability. Different colors were used to provide the four levels of irrelevant information; 240 Ss (10 in each of the 24 experimental conditions) made absolute judgments of size under the classical procedure for the method of single stimuli. Separate analyses of errors, reaction times, and rates of information transmission indicated that irrelevant information had an increasingly detrimental effect on absolute judgments as discrimination difficulty was increased (in the LD subgroup), but essentially no effect where discriminations were easily modefin the HD subgroup). 相似文献
12.
Tyron Craig Goldschmidt 《Philosophia》2011,39(2):267-288
Timothy O’Connor presents a novel and powerful version of the cosmological argument from contingency. What distinguishes his
argument is that it does not depend on the Principle of Sufficient Reason. This version thus avoids powerful objections facing
the Principle. We present and develop the argument, strengthening it in various ways. We fill in big gaps in the argument
and answer criticisms. These include the criticisms that O’Connor considers as well as new criticisms. We explain how his
replies to a Kantian criticism and to the demand for contrastive explanation fail, and properly answer the criticism and the
demand. We develop two new criticisms, the objection from opaqueness and the objection from constitution, and explain how
these objections can be answered. 相似文献
13.
Journal of Child and Family Studies - During adolescence, there are important changes in both depressive symptoms and self-concept; thus, the present study aimed to examine the effects of a... 相似文献
14.
ObjectivesThe present study explored the differences between athletes’ and parents’ perceptions of parental practices (i.e., active involvement, directive, pressure, praise, and understanding behaviours implemented by parents in the context of their child’s sport) by considering athletes’ and parents’ gender.Designcross-sectional study.MethodParents (N = 352) and athletes (N = 256, M = 14.72 years) completed a questionnaire to measure their perceptions of parental practices in sport. Zero-order correlations were computed for the entire sample and each sub-group (i.e., father-daughter, father-son, mother-daughter, and mother-son). One-level multilevel model (level 1: respondent) was computed to measure the influence of the respondent on the perceptions of parental practices. Two-level multilevel model (level 1: respondent, level 2: dyad) estimated the impact of the athletes’ gender, parents’ gender, and their interaction on the differences in perceptions.ResultsCorrelations highlighted relatively modest concordance between perceptions of parents and adolescents. Multilevel models showed that compared to athletes, parents reported significantly less frequent use for directive behaviours (β = −0.29) and more frequent use for active involvement (β = −0.18) and praise and understanding (β = 0.27). Correlations and multilevel models showed that the differences between athletes’ and parents’ perceptions of parental practices differed according to gender. The differences in perceptions of the directive behaviours (β = −0.22) and active involvement (β = 0.22) were higher when the father was involved in the dyad compared to the mother. 相似文献
15.
Angus G. Craig John M. D. Thompson Rebecca Slykerman Clare Wall Rinki Murphy Edwin A. Mitchell Karen E. Waldie 《Journal of child and family studies》2018,27(11):3544-3553
A father’s presence in the family is important for promoting adaptive behavioral functioning in children. It is unknown however, if there is a critical time during infancy and childhood for such paternal presence and involvement to affect behaviour. Using data from the Auckland Birthweight Collaborative (ABC) study, we examined the amount of paternal presence children experienced through their first 11 years of life (measured as fathers’ time spent in the same household as their children) and its effect on their behavioral outcomes at 11 years of age. After controlling for potential confounds, children who whose fathers were minimally present (left between 0 and 3.5 years of age) were twice as likely to report clinically significant behavioural difficulties as those whose fathers were present throughout childhood. Those whose fathers were present for early childhood (left between 3.5 and 7 years of age) exhibited no significant differences in their behaviour at 11 years of age when compared to their peers whose fathers remained present. Mothers reported no significant changes in their children’s behavior. Findings suggested that paternal presence early in a child’s life might be most important with regard to promoting adaptive behavioural functioning as they age. 相似文献
16.
A quasi-experimental study examined some effects of cross-age tutoring on fourth grade tutors' and second grade tutees' ability to spell accurately and correct errors on their own rough drafts. Half of the participating tutors were trained in tutoring skills; half were not. All participants were given pre- and post-assessments. On dictated writing assessments, second graders improved significantly, although fourth graders did not. On a free writing assessment, both second and fourth graders had significant gain scores. Observations during tutoring sessions showed benefits for both tutors and tutees. 相似文献
17.
18.
Research indicates people’s decisions can sometimes be influenced by seemingly trivial differences in the framing (i.e., wording) of alternative options. The tendency to prefer risk averse options when framed positively and risky options
when framed negatively is known as the framing effect. The current study examined the susceptibility of school principals
to the framing effect. Additionally, analytical and intuitive decision styles, the degree to which one’s typical goal is to
maximize (rather than satisfice), gender, and years of experience as a principal were measured to assess whether they are
predictive of principals’ choices, and to test whether they moderate the effects of framing on choice. Seventy-one principals
completed six decision problems (framed either positively or negatively) and instruments assessing decision style, typical
decision goal, gender, and experience. Analyses demonstrated that principals are influenced by framing. Although the positively
and negatively framed versions of the decision problems were objectively identical, negative framing resulted in more risky
choices. Additionally, regardless of frame, men made more risky choices than women. There was no evidence that experience,
decision style, or the degree to which one’s typical decision goal was to maximize, decreased framing effects. Several potential
debiasing strategies are described, and limitations are noted. 相似文献
19.
The Mediating Role of Psychological Empowerment on the Relationships between P–O Fit,Job Satisfaction,and In-role Performance 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Brian T. Gregory M. David Albritton Talai Osmonbekov 《Journal of business and psychology》2010,25(4):639-647
Purpose
The direct relationships between person–organization fit (P–O fit) and multiple individual-level outcomes such as job satisfaction and in-role performance have been heavily studied in the extant literature; potential mediators of these relationships have been studied much less frequently. Consequently, a complete picture of the psychology surrounding P–O fit is missing. This research aims to begin to fill this gap in the literature. 相似文献20.
The purpose of the present study was to assess the effects of single- versus multiple-exemplar training with several artists’ paintings on graduate students’ stimulus generalization to novel paintings by the same artists. Six graduate students participated in this study. Participants studied decks of cards that depicted images of paintings by six different artists. Multiple-exemplar decks included three exemplars of three artists for a total of nine cards, and single-exemplar decks included single exemplars of three artists for a total of three cards. Participants “tested out” when ready following independent study with each deck and were required to score 100 % correct in order to move on to a generalization probe, during which participants were presented with novel exemplars. Overall, four of six participants performed better on the generalization probe after multiple-exemplar training, though in the case of three participants, only to a slight degree. These modest effects suggest that multiple-exemplar training may facilitate stimulus generalization, but that studying with single exemplars may allow for stimulus generalization as well. 相似文献