首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
小学儿童在空间概念中的内隐学习实验研究   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
黄小忠  田学红 《心理科学》2006,29(4):830-832
本研究以一、三、五年级小学儿童为被试,探查小学儿童在不同学习模式下空间概念的发展情况。研究结果如下:(1)不同学习模式下,儿童的认知发展呈现不同的趋势;(2)随认知任务难度的增加,不同学习模式的成绩出现差异;(3)不同学习模式下,不同难度任务的成绩存在差异。  相似文献   

2.
3.
肖少北  许尚侠 《心理科学》2001,24(6):743-743,754
汉字是一种与拼音文字不同的表意文字,它具有较强的图形特征,整体性和形象性突出,给人以直观的感觉。汉字学习是一项复杂的活动,不仅要分别认记形、音、义这3个因素,特别是字形本身的结构关系,还要建立形、音、义三者之间的统一联系,而且在信息处理匕兼有“形码”和“声码”两种加工方式。汉字的这些特点以及认知汉字的独特方式,必然会对学习主体的心理产生特殊的影响,而这种特殊的影响可能从儿童学习和使用汉字开始就发生。因此.从发生的角度去探讨汉字学习对儿童思  相似文献   

4.
本研究运用结构性观察方法对两组4岁儿童与父母在四种活动中的互动进行了分析与比较。一组儿童为书面数符号测查高分者,二组儿童为书面数符号测查低分者。85对父母一儿童对子参加了4个各有15分钟的共同活动,阅读、数学题、纸和积木,并完成一个微型问卷。结果表明,在这些活动中包含了多种数学知识学习和多种以父母为主导的互动策略;两组对子之间在数学事件频次上无差异,但一组父母更多地运用了一些积极策略,二组父母更多地运用了一些消极策略;二组中有更多儿童对互动表现出消极回应和注意力问题。两个组的父母在家中与儿童一起进行诸如阅读和做数学题的共同活动的频次与质量上表现出差异。  相似文献   

5.
A key component of critical thinking is the ability to evaluate the statements of other people. Because information that is obtained from others is not always accurate, it is important that children learn to reason about it critically. By as early as age 3, children understand that people sometimes communicate inaccurate information and that some individuals are more reliable sources than others. However, in many contexts, even older children fail to evaluate sources critically. Recent research points to the role of social experience in explaining why children often fail to engage in critical reasoning.  相似文献   

6.
本研究采用视频信息加工任务.考察在无指导的自然阅读状态下.字幕在小学一,三年级儿童伴随学习中词汇获得的作用.研究结果表明:(1)在蒯读有字幕视频材料任务中.一、三年级儿童均可伴随学习到字幕中的生字,并且在高生字密度条件下习得的生字量要显著多于低生字密度条件;(2)一年级低阅读水平儿童在全文注音字幕条件习得的生字量与高阅读水平一致,但在无注音条件则显著低于高阅读水平;(3)三年级儿童在有拼音与无拼音字幕条件习得的生字量无显著差异.这表明利用字幕伴随学习,是一种适于儿童获得词汇知识的方法.  相似文献   

7.
Learning names for parts of objects can be challenging for children, as it requires overcoming their tendency to name whole objects. We test whether comparing items can facilitate learning names for their parts. Applying the structure-mapping theory of comparison leads to two predictions: (a) young children will find it easier to identify a common part between two very similar items than between two dissimilar items (because the similar pair is easier to align); (b) close alignments potentiate far alignments: children will be better able to extend a novel part name to a dissimilar object after having extended it to a similar object. In three studies, 227 preschool children mapped novel part terms to new animals or objects. Both predictions were confirmed. Children more accurately extended novel part terms to objects that were similar to the standard than to objects that were dissimilar (Experiments 1 and 2), and children more accurately extended novel part names to dissimilar objects after having extended them to similar objects (Experiment 3). We conclude that structure-mapping processes can support part learning.  相似文献   

8.
学习支持对儿童在多媒体语境中学习英语词汇的影响   总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4  
孙燕青  董奇 《心理科学》2003,26(5):800-803
该研究旨在考察中文翻译与主题提示两种学习支持对儿童在多媒体语境中学习英语词汇的作用,为此设计了三种学习条件:无支持条件、中文翻译条件和中文翻译 主题提示条件。对学龄初期儿童在上述三种学习条件下学习英语词汇的过程和结果进行的考察和分析表明:儿童在无支持条件下的学习效果较差;中文翻译可以明显促进儿童对语句的理解,但无法提高儿童对目标词的掌握;中文翻译 主题提示条件下儿童对单词语义的掌握最好,他们表现出了较多的跟读行为,并更倾向于认为学习任务较为容易。  相似文献   

9.
Looking away from an interlocutor's face during demanding cognitive activity can help adults and children answer challenging mental arithmetic and verbal-reasoning questions (Glenberg, Schroeder, & Robertson, 1998 Glenberg, A. M., Schroeder, J. L. and Robertson, D. A. 1998. Averting the gaze disengages the environment and facilitates remembering. Memory and Cognition, 26: 651658. [Crossref], [PubMed], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar]; Phelps, Doherty-Sneddon, & Warnock, 2006 Phelps, F., Doherty-Sneddon, G. and Warnock, H. 2006. Functional benefits of children's gaze aversion during questioning. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24: 577588. [Crossref], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar]). While such “gaze aversion” (GA) is used far less by 5-year-old school children, its use increases dramatically during the first years of primary education, reaching adult levels by 8 years of age (Doherty-Sneddon, Bruce, Bonner, Longbotham, & Doyle, 2002 Doherty-Sneddon, G., Bruce, V., Bonner, L., Longbotham, S. and Doyle, C. 2002. Development of gaze aversion as disengagement from visual information. Developmental Psychology, 38: 438445. [Crossref], [PubMed], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar]). Furthermore, GA increases with increasing mental demands, with high levels signalling that an individual finds material being discussed challenging but remains engaged with it (Doherty-Sneddon et al., 2002 Doherty-Sneddon, G., Bruce, V., Bonner, L., Longbotham, S. and Doyle, C. 2002. Development of gaze aversion as disengagement from visual information. Developmental Psychology, 38: 438445. [Crossref], [PubMed], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar]; Doherty-Sneddon & Phelps, 2005 Doherty-Sneddon, G. and Phelps, F. 2005. Gaze aversion: A solution to cognitive or social difficulty?. Memory and Cognition, 33: 727733. [Crossref], [PubMed], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar]). In the current study we investigate whether patterns of gaze and gaze aversion during children's explanations can predict when they are in states of transient knowledge (Karmiloff-Smith 1992 Karmiloff-Smith, A. 1992. Beyond modularity: A developmental perspective on cognitive science, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.  [Google Scholar]; Goldin-Meadow, Kim, & Singer, 1999 Goldin-Meadow, S., Kim, S. and Singer, M. 1999. What the teacher's hands tell the student's mind about math. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91: 720730. [Crossref], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar]). In Study 1, thirty-three 6-year-old children completed a balance beam task (Pine & Messer, 2000 Pine, K. J. and Messer, D. 2000. The effects of explaining another's actions on children's implicit theories of balance. Cognition and Instruction, 18: 3754. [Taylor & Francis Online] [Google Scholar]). Children who improved the representational level of their explanations (Karmiloff-Smith, 1992 Karmiloff-Smith, A. 1992. Beyond modularity: A developmental perspective on cognitive science, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.  [Google Scholar]) of this task with training used more GA than those who did not. Practical implications for teaching and for recognizing transient knowledge states are discussed. In Study 2, fifty-nine 6-year-olds took part and completed a “Time Task” along with periodic teaching intervention to improve their comprehension of telling the time. Some children improved immediately, whereas others did so more gradually. The gradual improvers showed the highest levels of GA, particularly when they were at an intermediate level of performance.  相似文献   

10.
使用结构访谈法对120名儿童(其中学习不良儿童60名)的策略信念与策略理解水平进行了研究。结果显示:学习不良儿童组与一般儿童组的差异更多体现在对具体策略的元认知因果解释水平上,他们更多持有较低水平的“信息获得”解释,而对照组儿童则更多持有较高水平的“信息加工”解释,提示对策略理解水平的差异可能是导致学习不良儿童难以将新学到的策略主动应用到其它情景中去的一个重要原因。  相似文献   

11.
当前儿童交往归因研究的特点   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
冯维  赵海钧 《心理科学》2004,27(3):757-759
交往归因是影响儿童交往的重要认知因素。近来.随着认知研究的深入.交往归因日趋成为交往机制研究的重要课题。从已有资料看来,儿童交往归因研究具有如下几个特点:多元视角研究的进行;研究方法力求科学化;改善交往归因不良状况的归因训练开始受到重视等。  相似文献   

12.
儿童英语句法学习中内隐学习和外显学习的关系   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
《心理科学》2006,29(6):1364-1369
本文以内隐学习理论为基础,采用强分离程序,分别选取有、无英语学习经验的被试,从句法学习方面探讨了儿童英语学习过程中内隐学习和外显学习的作用及关系,结果表明:(1)单纯的内隐学习方式或单纯的外显学习方式都不利于对句法规则的掌握;(2)两种学习方式的协同效应发挥着重要的作用,先外显学习后内隐学习的学习方式有助于句法规则的掌握;(3)内隐学习、先内隐学习后外显学习的学习方式不适用于较简单句法规则的学习;内隐学习也不适用于难度较高的句法规则的学习。  相似文献   

13.
儿童对假装的认知发展的研究进展   总被引:6,自引:0,他引:6  
王桂琴  方格  杨小冬 《心理科学》2003,26(5):898-900
1 引言  近二十年来 ,儿童心理理论 (theoryofmind)的发展一直是一个非常重要的研究领域 ,在认知发展研究领域里占有主导地位。心理理论发展的研究主要探查了儿童最基本的心理状态方面的知识 ,这方面的一个关键问题是学前儿童何时能理解心理是对客观世界的表征 ,这种表征有时可能与客观现实不同。儿童对心理表征的理解的研究主要集中于儿童对错误信念的认知 ,儿童是否认识到人的信念并不总是与客观现实一致 ,人的信念可能错误地反映了客观现实。关于错误信念的研究已取得丰硕的成果 ,实验证明儿童在 4岁以后已能理解错误信念。据此研究者…  相似文献   

14.
Children's goals for learning on their video game performance and patterns of attention were examined. Before playing a game, second- and fifth-graders were instructed to adopt an evaluative, process, outcome, or no specific goal focus while playing. Children were then interviewed about their game strategies and the game features they paid attention to while playing. Older children and more frequent players showed better performance. Among frequent players, process goal instructions facilitated performance. Younger children's interview references to process goals also were predictive of better performance. Their references to attention strategies, however, were predictive of poorer performance while older children's references to attention strategies were predictive of better performance. These findings highlight the efficacy of process goals for learning among younger and older children.  相似文献   

15.
Learning From Social Studies Texts   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
《认知与教导》2013,31(2):99-158
  相似文献   

16.
The authors examined the process of change in size comparison strategies among preschool-age children. Twelve 5- and 6-year-old children were provided with origami exercises and size comparison tasks for 5 days consecutively, and another twelve 5- and 6-year-old children received size comparison tasks only. Children's strategies for the size comparison tasks were analyzed in terms of placement and adjustment. In contrast with the children in the control condition, the children who were provided with exercises in origami increased their use of superimposition and adjustment strategies by 2 dimensions across the 5 days. The authors argue that origami exercises helped the children connect their implicit understanding of relative size with the use of more effective strategies for size comparison.  相似文献   

17.
This study examines whether 3-year-olds are able to use ontological distinctions in homophone and nonce word acquisition. In the homophone (denotation-1 present) condition, children heard stories introducing second meanings of homophones; for example, a skate (i.e., fish). Stories contained perceptual information, information about theory-based properties, or filler material. Children identified the referent from 3 drawings depicting the familiar meaning, the secondary meaning, and a distractor. In the homophone (denotation-1 absent) condition, the picture of the familiar meaning was replaced with a distractor. The nonce word condition, a control condition, was identical to the homophone (denotation-1 present) condition except that a nonce word replaced the homophone. Children were rarely able to construct a second homophone interpretation when the familiar denotation was available. Interestingly, in the remaining conditions, children used information about the animate-inanimate distinction in choosing the referent. We discuss children's use of this ontological distinction when inducing word meaning.  相似文献   

18.
国外儿童认知发展与学习的最新研究述评   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
佟秀丽  莫雷  Zhe Chen 《心理科学》2006,29(3):735-739
认知发展变化是儿童发展研究的中心课题,而儿童学习是研究儿童认知发展变化的一个重要方面。20世纪90年代以来,随着儿童学习重新研究,儿童认知发展研究取得显著进展。本文在最新儿童认知发展和学习研究的基础上,概述儿童早期认知能力的新发现以及儿童学习的有关研究和结论,并提出了新的研究框架。  相似文献   

19.
9~11岁儿童对失败学习情景的情绪反应和情绪表达研究   总被引:6,自引:0,他引:6  
徐琴美  鞠晓辉 《心理科学》2005,28(2):447-450
本研究对180名9-11岁儿童在失败学习情景下的情绪反应和情绪表达进行了研究。结果表明:(1)儿童在失败学习情景中情绪表达的意愿受年龄影响。(2)儿童在失败学习情景中的情绪表达原因受年龄和观众背景影响。儿童隐藏情绪的原因主要是害怕消极的后果,表达情绪的原因是为了得到积极的结果。(3)儿童在失败学习情景中情绪表达和隐藏的策略受年龄、性别和观众背景的影响。(4)儿童的后继行为受年龄的影响。  相似文献   

20.
李红  白琼英 《心理科学》2005,28(2):277-280
与Brenmer等(1997)的实验对照,对4~7岁的儿童考察观察角度的变化方式、模型状态、年龄等与其绘画简单深度关系中视觉写实水平的关系。实验结果表明:年龄是影响儿童绘画简单深度关系中视觉写实水平的重要因素;观察角度变化的两种方式在6~7岁组有显著差异;模型状态对不同性别儿童的绘画成绩影响显著。  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号