首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
In delay eyeblink conditioning, the CS overlaps with the US and only a brainstem-cerebellar circuit is necessary for learning. In trace eyeblink conditioning, the CS ends before the US is delivered and several forebrain structures, including the hippocampus, are required for learning, in addition to a brainstem-cerebellar circuit. The interstimulus interval (ISI) between CS onset and US onset is perhaps the most important factor in classical conditioning, but studies comparing delay and trace conditioning have typically not matched these procedures in this crucial factor, so it is often difficult to determine whether results are due to differences between delay and trace or to differences in ISI. In the current study, we employed a 580-ms CS-US interval for both delay and trace conditioning and compared hippocampal CA1 activity and cerebellar interpositus nucleus activity in order to determine whether a unique signature of trace conditioning exists in patterns of single-unit activity in either structure. Long-Evans rats were chronically implanted in either CA1 or interpositus with microwire electrodes and underwent either delay eyeblink conditioning, or trace eyeblink conditioning with a 300-ms trace period between CS offset and US onset. On trials with a CR in delay conditioning, CA1 pyramidal cells showed increases in activation (relative to a pre-CS baseline) during the CS-US period in sessions 1-4 that was attenuated by sessions 5-6. In contrast, on trials with a CR in trace conditioning, CA1 pyramidal cells did not show increases in activation during the CS-US period until sessions 5-6. In sessions 5-6, increases in activation were present only to the CS and not during the trace period. For rats with interpositus electrodes, activation of interpositus neurons on CR trials was present in all sessions in both delay and trace conditioning. However, activation was greater in trace compared to delay conditioning in the first half of the CS-US interval (during the trace CS) during early sessions of conditioning and, in later sessions of conditioning, activation was greater in the second half of the CS-US interval (during the trace interval). These results suggest that the pattern of hippocampal activation that differentiates trace from delay eyeblink conditioning is a slow buildup of activation to the CS, possibly representing encoding of CS duration or discrimination of the CS from the background context. Interpositus nucleus neurons show strong modeling of the eyeblink CR regardless of paradigm but show a changing pattern across conditioning that may be due to the necessary contributions of forebrain processing to trace conditioning.  相似文献   

2.
Young adult Purkinje cell degeneration (pcd) mutant mice, with complete loss of cerebellar cortical Purkinje cells, are impaired in delay eyeblink classical conditioning. In the delay paradigm, the conditioned stimulus (CS) overlaps and coterminates with the unconditioned stimulus (US), and the cerebellar cortex supports normal acquisition. The ability of pcd mutant mice to acquire trace eyeblink conditioning in which the CS and US do not overlap has not been explored. Recent evidence suggests that cerebellar cortex may not be necessary for trace eyeblink classical conditioning. Using a 500 ms trace paradigm for which forebrain structures are essential in mice, we assessed the performance of homozygous male pcd mutant mice and their littermates in acquisition and extinction. In contrast to results with delay conditioning, acquisition of trace conditioning was unimpaired in pcd mutant mice. Extinction to the CS alone did not differ between pcd and littermate control mice, and timing of the conditioned response was not altered by the absence of Purkinje cells during acquisition or extinction. The ability of pcd mutant mice to acquire and extinguish trace eyeblink conditioning at levels comparable to controls suggests that the cerebellar cortex is not a critical component of the neural circuitry underlying trace conditioning. Results indicate that the essential neural circuitry for trace eyeblink conditioning involves connectivity that bypasses cerebellar cortex.  相似文献   

3.
The current study examined the effects of globally depleting Purkinje cells in the cerebellar cortex with the immunotoxin OX7-saporin on acquisition and extinction of delay eyeblink conditioning in rats. Rats were given OX7-saporin or saline 2 wk before the start of eyeblink conditioning. The rats that reached a performance criterion of two consecutive days with 80% or greater conditioned responses were given 5 d of extinction training followed by 2 d of reacquisition training. Rats that received infusions of OX7-saporin had 77.2%-97.9% Purkinje cell loss and exhibited impaired acquisition and extinction. The amount of Purkinje cell loss was correlated with the magnitude of the acquisition and extinction impairments. The highest correlations between Purkinje cell number and the rate of acquisition were in lobule HVI and the anterior lobe. The highest negative correlation between Purkinje cell number and the percentage of conditioned responses during extinction was in the anterior lobe. The results indicate that cerebellar Purkinje cells, particularly in the anterior lobe and lobule HVI, play significant roles in acquisition and extinction of eyeblink conditioning.  相似文献   

4.
Rabbits received lesions of the mediodorsal nucleus of the thalamus (MDN) or sham lesions and were subjected to classical eyeblink (EB) and heart rate (HR) conditioning. All animals received trace conditioning, with a.5-sec tone conditioned stimulus, a .5-sec trace period, and a 50-msec periorbital shock unconditioned stimulus. Animals with MDN lesions acquired the EB conditioned response (CR) more slowly than sham-lesioned animals. However, previous studies have shown that MDN damage does not affect delay conditioning using either .5-sec or 1-sec interstimulus intervals. The lesions had no significant effect on the HR CR. These results suggest that information processed by MDN and relayed to the prefrontal cortex is required for somatomotor response selection under nonoptimal learning conditions.  相似文献   

5.
Autism is unique among other disorders in that acquisition of conditioned eyeblink responses is enhanced in children, occurring in a fraction of the trials required for control participants. The timing of learned responses is, however, atypical. Two animal models of autism display a similar phenotype. Researchers have hypothesized that these differences in conditioning reflect cerebellar abnormalities. The present study used computer simulations of the cerebellar cortex, including inhibition by the molecular layer interneurons, to more closely examine whether atypical cerebellar processing can account for faster conditioning in individuals with autism. In particular, the effects of inhibitory levels on delay eyeblink conditioning were simulated, as were the effects of learning-related synaptic changes at either parallel fibers or ascending branch synapses from granule cells to Purkinje cells. Results from these simulations predict that whether molecular layer inhibition results in an enhancement or an impairment of acquisition, or changes in timing, may depend on (1) the sources of inhibition, (2) the levels of inhibition, and (3) the locations of learning-related changes (parallel vs. ascending branch synapses). Overall, the simulations predict that a disruption in the balance or an overall increase of inhibition within the cerebellar cortex may contribute to atypical eyeblink conditioning in children with autism and in animal models of autism.  相似文献   

6.
Neuronal activity was recorded in the pontine nuclei of developing rats during eyeblink conditioning on postnatal days 17–18 (P17–P18) or P24–P25. A pretraining session consisted of unpaired presentations of a 300-msec tone conditioned stimulus (CS) and a 10-msec periorbital shock unconditioned stimulus (US). Five paired training sessions followed the unpaired session, consisting of 100 trials of the CS paired with the US. The rats trained on P24–P25 exhibited significantly more conditioned responses (CRs) than the rats trained on P17–P18, although both groups produced CRs by the end of training. Ontogenetic increases in pre-CS and stimulus-elicited activity in the pontine nuclei were observed during the pretraining session and after paired training. The activity of pontine units was greater on trials with CRs relative to trials without CRs in rats trained on P24–P25, but almost no CR-related modulation was observed in the pontine units of rats trained on P17–P18. The findings indicate that pontine neuronal responses to the CS and modulation of pontine activity by the cerebellum and red nucleus undergo substantial postnatal maturation. The developmental changes in pontine neuronal activity might play a significant role in the ontogeny of eyeblink conditioning.  相似文献   

7.
A growing body of literature suggests that structures along the midline of the prefrontal cortex (mPFC), including Brodmann’s area 32 (prelimbic cortex) and area 24 (anterior cingulate cortex) in the rabbit play a role in retrieval of learned information. The present studies compared the effects of post-training lesions produced either immediately or 1-week following learning, to either prelimbic (area 32) or anterior cingulate (area 24) cortex on trace eyeblink (EB) conditioning. Further, because recent evidence suggests that the mPFC may play an even greater role in learning and memory when emotional arousal is low, these studies compared the effects of lesions in groups conditioned with either a relatively low-arousal corneal airpuff, or a more aversive periorbital eyeshock unconditioned stimulus (US). A total of six groups were tested, which received selective ibotenic acid or “sham” control lesions to either area 32 or 24, immediately or 1-week following asymptotic learning, and conditioned with an eyeshock US or an airpuff US. Results showed that the greatest lesion deficits were found when conditioning with the less aversive airpuff US. Further, lesions produced to area 32 one-week, but not immediately following learning, caused significant deficits in performance, while lesions produced to area 24 immediately, but not 1-week following learning, caused significant deficits in performance. These findings add to the body of evidence which shows that area 32 of the mPFC regulates retrieval, but not acquisition or storage of information, while area 24 mediates a less specific reacquisition process, but not permanent storage or retrieval of information during relearning of memories abolished by mPFC damage. These findings were, however, specific to those experiments in which the relatively non-aversive airpuff was the US.  相似文献   

8.
How the nervous system encodes learning and memory processes has interested researchers for 100 years. Over this span of time, a number of basic neuroscience methods has been developed to explore the relationship between learning and the brain, including brain lesion, stimulation, pharmacology, anatomy, imaging, and recording techniques. In this paper, we summarize how different research approaches can be employed to generate converging data that speak to how structures and systems in the brain are involved in simple associative learning. To accomplish this, we review data regarding the involvement of a particular region of cerebellar cortex (Larsell's lobule HVI) in the widely used paradigm of classical eyeblink conditioning. We also present new data on the role of lobule HVI in eyeblink conditioning generated by combining temporary brain inactivation and single-cell recording methods, an approach that looks promising for further advancing our understanding of relationships between brain and behavior.  相似文献   

9.
The rodent eyeblink conditioning paradigm is an ideal model system for examining the relationship between neural maturation and the ontogeny of associative learning. Elucidation of the neural mechanisms underlying the ontogeny of learning is tractable using eyeblink conditioning because the necessary neural circuitry (cerebellum and interconnected brainstem nuclei) underlying the acquisition and retention of the conditioned response (CR) has been identified in adult organisms. Moreover, the cerebellum exhibits substantial postnatal anatomical and physiological maturation in rats. The eyeblink CR emerges developmentally between postnatal day (PND) 17 and 24 in rats. A series of experiments found that the ontogenetic emergence of eyeblink conditioning is related to the development of associative learning and not related to changes in performance. More recent studies have examined the relationship between the development of eyeblink conditioning and the physiological maturation of the cerebellum, a brain structure that is necessary for eyeblink conditioning in adult organisms. Disrupting cerebellar development with lesions or antimitotic treatments impairs the ontogeny of eyeblink conditioning. Studies of the development of physiological processes within the cerebellum have revealed striking ontogenetic changes in stimulus-elicited and learning-related neuronal activity. Neurons in the interpositus nucleus and Purkinje cells in the cortex exhibit developmental increases in neuronal discharges following the unconditioned stimulus (US) and in neuronal discharges that model the amplitude and time-course of the eyeblink CR. The developmental changes in CR-related neuronal activity in the cerebellum suggest that the ontogeny of eyeblink conditioning depends on the development of mechanisms that estavlish cerebellar plasticity. Learning and the induction of neural plasticity depend on the magnitude of the US input to the cerebellum. The role of developmental changes in the efficacy of the US pathway has been investigated by monitoring neuronal activity in the inferior olive and with stimulation techniques. The results of these experiments indicate that the development of the conditioned eyeblink response may depend on dynamic interactions between multiple developmental processes within the eyeblink neural circuitry.  相似文献   

10.
The learning of an association between a CS and a US can be retarded by unreinforced presentations of the CS alone (termed latent inhibition or LI) or by un-correlated presentations of the CS and US (termed learned irrelevance or LIRR). In rabbit eyeblink conditioning, there have been some recent failures to replicate LI. LIRR has been hypothesized as producing a stronger retardation effect than LI based on both empirical studies and computational models. In the work presented here, we examined the relative strength of LI and LIRR in eyeblink conditioning in rabbits and humans. In both species, a number of pre-exposure trials sufficient to produce LIRR failed to produce LI (Experiments 1 & 3). Doubling the number of CS pre-exposures did produce LI in rabbits (Experiment 2), but not in humans (Experiment 4). LI was demonstrated in humans only after manipulations including an increased inter-trial interval or ITI (Experiment 5). Overall, it appears that LIRR is a more easily producible pre-exposure retardation effect than LI for eyeblink conditioning in both rabbits and humans. Several theoretical mechanisms for LI including the conditioned attention theory, stimulus compression, novelty, and the switching theory are discussed as possible explanations for the differences between LIRR and LI. Overall, future work involving testing the neural substrates of pre-exposure effects may benefit from the use of LIRR rather than LI.  相似文献   

11.
The neural plasticity necessary for acquisition and retention of eyeblink conditioning has been localized to the cerebellum. However, the sources of sensory input to the cerebellum that are necessary for establishing learning-related plasticity have not been identified completely. The inferior colliculus may be a source of sensory input to the cerebellum through its projection to the medial auditory thalamus. The medial auditory thalamus is necessary for eyeblink conditioning in rats and projects to the lateral pontine nuclei, which then project to the cerebellar nuclei and cortex. The current experiment examined the role of the inferior colliculus in auditory eyeblink conditioning. Rats were given bilateral or unilateral (contralateral to the conditioned eye) lesions of the inferior colliculus prior to 10 d of delay eyeblink conditioning with a tone CS. Rats with bilateral or unilateral lesions showed equivalently impaired acquisition. The extent of damage to the contralateral inferior colliculus correlated with several measures of conditioning. The findings indicate that the contralateral inferior colliculus provides auditory input to the cerebellum that is necessary for eyeblink conditioning.  相似文献   

12.
The classical conditioning task of blocking involves the adding of a novel but redundant stimulus to a previously trained stimulus. Both blocking and novelty detection are thought to involve the hippocampus. Previously, Solomon (1977) found that nonselective aspiration lesions of the hippocampal region eliminated blocking in rabbit eyeblink conditioning. We tested the effects of selective ibotenic acid lesions of the hippocampus on blocking, as well as on novelty detection, when training is switched from a tone conditioned stimulus (CS) to a compound tone-light CS in eyeblink conditioning. Selective hippocampal lesions did not eliminate blocking but did lead to a facilitation of conditioned response (CR) acquisition to the tone and to the light, but not to the tone-light compound. Selective hippocampal lesions disrupted a CR decrement observed in sham surgical controls when transferred from tone training to tone-light training. It appears that although selective hippocampal lesions do not eliminate blocking in eyeblink conditioning, they do disrupt novelty detection and may facilitate learning to a previously blocked cue.  相似文献   

13.
In 1978, Berry and Thompson showed that the amount of theta (3–8 Hz) activity in the spontaneous hippocampal EEG predicted learning rate in subsequent eyeblink conditioning in rabbits. More recently, the absence of theta activity during the training trial has been shown to have a detrimental effect on learning rate. Here, we aimed to further explore the relationship between theta activity and classical eyeblink conditioning by determining how the relative power of hippocampal theta activity [theta/(theta + delta) ratio] changes during both unpaired control and paired training phases. We found that animals with a higher hippocampal theta ratio immediately before conditioning learned faster and also that in these animals the theta ratio was higher throughout both experimental phases. In fact, while the hippocampal theta ratio remained stable in the fast learners as a function of training, it decreased in the slow learners already during unpaired training. In addition, the presence of hippocampal theta activity enhanced the hippocampal model of the conditioned response (CR) and seemed to be beneficial for CR performance in terms of peak latency during conditioning, but did not have any effect when the animals showed asymptotic learning. Together with earlier findings, these results imply that the behavioral state in which hippocampal theta activity is absent is detrimental for learning, and that the behavioral state in which hippocampal theta activity dominates is beneficial for learning, at least before a well-learned state is achieved.  相似文献   

14.
Big-K+ conductance (BK)-channel mediated fast afterhyperpolarizations (AHPs) following action potentials are reduced after eyeblink conditioning. Blocking BK channels with paxilline increases evoked firing frequency in vitro and spontaneous pyramidal activity in vivo. To examine how increased excitability after BK-channel blockade affects learning, rats received bilateral infusions of paxilline, saline, or nothing into hippocampal CA1 prior to trace eyeblink conditioning. The drug group was slower to acquire the task, but learning was not completely impaired. This suggests that nonspecific increases in excitability and baseline neuronal firing rates caused by in vivo blockade of the BK channel may disrupt correct processing of inputs, thereby impairing hippocampus-dependent learning.Learning and increased neuronal intrinsic excitability are strongly correlated, although a causal relationship has not yet been definitively established (Disterhoft and Oh 2006). One of the mechanisms of increased excitability is through reduction of potassium currents, which cause afterhyperpolarizations (AHP). Afterhyperpolarizations in pyramidal cells can be divided into three categories based upon their timing and duration. The fast AHP lasts only 2–5 ms, follows the depolarizing phase of individual action potentials, and is mediated largely by the big-K+ conductance (BK) channel. The post-burst AHP has a medium (50–100 ms) and a slow (1–2 s) component, and follows trains or bursts of action potentials. The medium AHP is carried by apamin sensitive small-K+ conductance (SK) channels, but the channel(s), which carry the slow AHP, are still unknown (Storm 1987; Disterhoft and Oh 2006).Learning-related reductions in the post-burst AHP are well documented (for review see Disterhoft and Oh 2006). Additionally, pharmacological modulators of the post-burst AHP alter learning in an expected manner—compounds that reduce the AHP improve learning (galantamine [Simon et al. 2004] and nimodopine [Deyo et al. 1989]). Learning-related reductions of the fast AHP are also seen in prefrontal cortex pyramidal neurons after extinction of fear conditioning (Santini et al. 2008) and in CA1 hippocampal pyramidal neurons after learning trace eyeblink conditioning (tEBC) (Matthews et al. 2008). In vitro whole-cell recordings show that blocking the BK channel with either paxilline or iberiotoxin increases the firing rate to a step current injection (Nelson et al. 2003). Likewise, injection of paxilline into the hippocampus increases the in vivo spontaneous firing frequency of hippocampal CA1 neurons of awake freely moving rats up to 2.5-fold over saline injections (Matthews et al. 2008), indicating that the BK-mediated fast AHP plays an important role in intrinsic excitability. The current study was undertaken to determine whether pharmacologically reducing the fast AHP during training would improve trace eyeblink conditioning.Experimental subjects were 3- to 4-mo old Fisher 344 X Brown Norway F1-hybrid rats. Animals were housed in pairs, in a climate-controlled facility with a 12:12 light–dark cycle, and ad libitum access to food and water. Procedures were in accordance with the guidelines of the Northwestern University Animal Care and Use Committee and conformed to NIH standards (NIH Publication No. 80-23). All efforts were made to minimize the number of animals utilized and their discomfort. Thirty-seven rats were originally included in the study, however 13 were excluded from the experiment due to incorrect cannulae placement, faulty EMG signal, or unrelated health issues. The final groups included in the study were nine drug animals, seven vehicle animals, eight sham animals, and six non-cannulated animals.Guide cannulae (26 gauge stainless steel) were bilaterally implanted at −3.6 mm AP, ±2.0 mm ML. The guide cannulae were lowered slowly (0.5 mm/5 min) to a depth of −1.9 mm subdura. The cannulae were cemented in place with dental acrylic. An electronic connector strip with pins for ground and two EMG wires was fitted between the two guide cannulae and grounded to the skull screws. The EMG wires were implanted under the right eyelid and the entire apparatus was cemented in place (Weiss et al. 1999). Rats were given Buprenex (0.05 mg/kg) post-surgery to alleviate any discomfort.Cannulae placements were verified histologically after training was completed. Animals were given a lethal dose of barbiturate (0.15 mL i.p.) then transcardially perfused with 0.9% saline followed by 10% formalin. After perfusion, the brains were carefully immersed in 10% formalin for a minimum of 24 h. Eighty micron coronal sections were made with a freezing cryostat, and every second section was kept and mounted on gelatin coated slides. Sections were stained with cresyl violet to reveal cell death or excessive damage surrounding the injection cannulae. Animals with incorrectly placed cannulae or excessive tissue damage were not included in the study. A diagram of the most ventral extent of each cannula is shown in Figure 1A. Paxilline, a BK-channel blocker, is a peptide and there is little known about the motility of this molecule in the brain. From previous in vivo recording work (Matthews et al. 2008), it is known that the BK blocker is active and able to diffuse at least a radius of 1.7 mm from the tip of the cannula. To approximate the spread of paxilline in the present study, 1.0% ibotenic acid was injected into two animals at the completion of training in a manner that exactly mimicked the paxilline injections in volume and rate. Five days after excitotoxic lesion, animals were sacrificed and their brains processed. Figure 1B shows the maximum and minimum extent of cell loss due to ibotenic acid lesion.Open in a separate windowFigure 1.Cannula placement and drug spread. (A) Cannulae were bilaterally implanted to terminate directly above the CA1 layer of the hippocampus. Placement was verified after the behavioral experiments. Gray dots indicate the location of the tip of each cannula for animals included in the study. (B) The spread of the drug was approximated by injecting 1.0% ibotenic acid into the left hemisphere of two animals at the end of training. The injection volume and rate were the same as those used for the study. The right hemisphere served as a within-animal control for the action of the ibotenic acid. The maximal (light gray) and minimal (dark gray) spread are shown. Measurements are relative to bregma. (Adapted from Paxinos and Watson [1998] and reprinted with permission from Elsevier ©1998.)Freely moving animals were injected with vehicle (1% DMSO in saline), drug (1 μM paxilline in saline), or nothing 20–30 min before the start of training, including the first habituation session. The trainer was blind to the identity of the injected substance. Infusions were performed using two 2 μL Hamilton syringes connected by lengths of flexible, oil filled tubing to 33 gauge infusion needles, which extended 0.5 mm beyond the end of the guide cannulae. One microliter of sterile solution was infused into each hemisphere at a constant rate of 0.2 μL/min using a Stoelting double-barrel infusion pump. The injection needles were left in place for 1 min following the injection to allow diffusion away from the tip of the needle.Trace eyeblink conditioning is an associative learning task in which a neutral conditioned stimulus (CS) is paired with an unconditioned stimulus (US) that elicits a reflexive eyelid closure. The insertion of a stimulus-free “trace” interval between the CS and the US makes this task strongly dependent on the hippocampus (Solomon et al. 1986; Moyer Jr. et al. 1990). After repeated pairings of the CS and the US, if an association has been learned, the subject will begin to blink during the trace period in anticipation of the US. Trace eyeblink conditioning procedures as described by Weiss et al. (1999) were followed. Training sessions were conducted in a sound-attenuating chamber and controlled with a custom-designed LabVIEW (National Instruments) program; eyelid EMG data were integrated online during training. Animals were connected to the recording computer via the implanted connector strip; a short tether served the dual purpose of allowing EMG activity to be monitored and positioning an air puff delivery tube in front of the eye, while the rat was freely moving. On the first day, the subjects received a session of stimulus-free habituation to the training chamber lasting as long as a conditioning session. The subsequent 5 d were training sessions. Conditioned animals received 30 trials per session (30 s average ITI) for a total of 150 CS–US pairings, consisting of a tone stimulus (CS, 80 dB, 250 ms) paired with a corneal air puff (US, 3–5 psi, 100 ms) with a 250 ms stimulus-free trace interval interposed.The primary measure of tEBC learning used in this study was a correctly timed eyelid closure, i.e., an eyelid closure that begins during the trace interval and continues until the air puff. Eyelid activity was measured with an implanted EMG electrode. The division of eyelid responses into “adaptive” and “nonadaptive” categories has been used in other studies (Garcia et al. 1999). For this reason, we analyzed eyelid closure during the entire tone/trace period and during only the last 200 ms preceding the air puff. Responses given during the 200 ms preceding the US are termed adaptive conditioned responses (CR). Figure 2 shows the timing of the tone, trace, and air puff; the timeline for each type of response; and an integrated EMG. Any trials in which the baseline activity in the 500 ms preceding CS presentation exceeded two standard deviations were discarded. Eye closure was defined as greater than four standard deviations above baseline. Averages for all relevant measures for each session were compared between groups for training sessions 1–5 using repeated-measures ANOVA. Learning across training sessions within each group was assessed with a planned comparison ANOVA using StatVIEW software. The stimulus-free habituation session was excluded from all between-group analyses. The percentage of aCRs during habituation is shown in Figure 3 to provide the baseline level of spontaneous eyelid closures.Open in a separate windowFigure 2.Timing of conditioned responses (CRs) in relation to the tone, trace, and air puff. The integrated EMG shows a robust adaptive eyelid closure in anticipation of the air puff. The tone lasted 250 ms and was followed by a 250 ms trace period. The air puff was 100 ms long. Below the EMG trace is a time line showing the period for an adaptive CR and an unconditioned response (UR).Open in a separate windowFigure 3.BK block slows learning of trace eyeblink conditioning. Vehicle, drug, and sham animals were injected and immediately trained on the trace eyeblink task over 6 d, one session each day. (Non-cannulated animals were trained in two sessions per day for 3 d.) The first session for all groups was stimuli-free habituation. Each session consisted of 30 pairings of a CS tone with a US air puff. Injection of the BK blocker paxilline resulted in slower acquisition of the task (*P < 0.05), although animals in all groups were eventually able to learn the task (#P < 0.001).The concentration of paxilline was selected based on the dose that achieved a maximal increase in spontaneous firing in vivo (Matthews et al. 2008). Two groups were designed to control for pressure effects of the injection or the stress and tissue damage of the cannulation surgery. These were a vehicle group (1% DMSO in saline) and a sham group (empty needles). In addition, the learning behavior of these cannulated animals was compared to a separate group of animals that had only the EBC head apparatus implanted (non-cannulated).Rats that received an infusion of paxilline (1 μM) immediately preceding training were significantly slower to acquire the task as measured by the percent of adaptive CRs exhibited across all training sessions (F (3,26) = 3.155, P = 0.042, repeated-measures ANOVA, Fisher''s PLSD, P < 0.02) (Fig. 3). The percentage of responses during the entire tone/trace period showed a trend toward reduced learning in the drug group (F (3,26) = 2.729, P = 0.064, repeated-measures ANOVA).Other parameters of the eyelid closure were examined. There were no significant differences in the peak, onset, duration, or area of the adaptive CR. There was also no difference between groups in the onset of the air-puff elicited eyelid closure (unconditioned response [UR]), suggesting that the drug did not cause decreased sensitivity to the US. Finally, the baseline eyelid EMG activity of all four groups during the stimulus-free habituation session was not significantly different, suggesting that the drug did not suppress or enhance spontaneous eyelid activity.All animals in the study showed improved performance across the training sessions (F (4,104) = 21.810, P < 0.0001, repeated-measures ANOVA). Further analyses revealed that animals in the drug group also showed continuous learning over the training days (Session 1: 16.1% ± 4.9%, Session 5: 56.8% ± 8.2%; F (4,32) = 6.961; P = 0.0004), and eventually the drug group reached a percentage of adaptive CRs statistically comparable to the controls (F (3,26) = 2.377, P = 0.093 for Session 5, ANOVA).There is a precedent for anticipating enhanced learning after in vivo pharmacological manipulations of intrinsic excitability (Disterhoft and Oh 2006). The long-lasting post-burst AHP is reduced in hippocampal cells from animals that have learned a hippocampus-dependent task (Moyer Jr. et al. 1996; Oh et al. 2003), and increased in aging animals that have difficulty learning. Pharmacologically reducing the post-burst AHP in vivo with calcium channel blockers (nimodipine) (Deyo et al. 1989), acetylcholine agonists (CI-1017) (Weiss et al. 2000), or cholinesterase inhibitors (galantamine and metrifonate) (Kronforst-Collins et al. 1997; Weible et al. 2004) leads to improved learning in aging animals. Thus, the finding that blocking the BK channel results in slowed learning is somewhat surprising, given the previous report of a reduction in the BK-mediated fast AHP after learning tEBC (Matthews et al. 2008). Several explanations may account for why pharmacologically reducing the fast AHP in vivo impaired rather than improved learning.First, the reduction of the fast AHP seen with channel blockers in in vitro experiments is of greater magnitude than the reductions in the fast AHP seen after learning; additionally, the fast AHP in cells from trained animals can be further reduced in vitro with paxilline or iberiotoxin (Matthews et al. 2008). It could be that there is an important difference between reducing the BK-mediated current, as is seen after learning, and completely blocking it using a drug. Reducing the fast AHP increases intrinsic excitability, and completely blocking the BK channel increases excitability even further. However, it is important to emphasize recent research showing how excessive excitability can be detrimental to learning. An early indicator of mild cognitive impairment detected with fMRI is hyperactivity in the hippocampus and medial temporal lobe (Miller et al. 2008). At the cellular level, saturating in vivo hippocampal LTP results in impaired spatial learning due to increased epileptiform activity, rather than saturated synaptic plasticity (McNamara et al. 1993). Finally, work with a knockout model of the β-4 subunit of the BK channel shows that this calcium- and voltage-dependent channel helps to regulate hyperexcitability and reduce the occurrence of temporal lobe seizures (Brenner et al. 2005). Although intrahippocampal paxilline infusion did not cause epileptiform activity in in vivo recordings, or observable seizure behavior (Juhng et al. 1999), it is possible that the drug caused pathological increases in excitability that impeded learning.Second, unregulated or meaningless increases in baseline neural activity in the hippocampus increase background noise, effectively decreasing the signal-to-noise ratio for the whole network, making it more difficult to distinguish important, information-bearing activity from background noise. In delay conditioning (where there is no trace interval between the CS and US), ablation of the hippocampus does not disrupt eyeblink conditioning (Solomon et al. 1986; Hesslow 1994), however, increasing (Salafia et al. 1979) or suppressing (Solomon et al. 1983) hippocampal activity has a strong retarding effect on learning this task. Disrupting synaptic transmission in a subregion of the hippocampus also impairs spatial learning (Niewoehner et al. 2007), further illustrating how a disturbance of information processing at a single junction of the trisynaptic circuit can impair learning. BK channels are present in presynaptic terminals, as well as at the soma, where they participate in controlling transmitter release. Blocking BK channels decreases failures and increases the amplitude of EPSCs at CA3–CA3 synapses (Raffaelli et al. 2004). The infusion of paxilline may have also increased the efficacy and frequency of transmitter release at the CA3–CA1 synapse in the present experiment. It may be that by selecting a dose of paxilline that caused a maximum increase in in vivo spontaneous activity, we overdosed the hippocampus to levels of excitability that interfered with stimulus processing, thereby impairing and slowing learning.Finally, the fast AHP is largely mediated by the BK channel, but other potassium currents also play a role in action potential repolarization. The A-type potassium current in particular has been implicated in learning-related excitability changes and is active during an action potential (Giese et al. 1998). Changing action potential repolarization dynamics also alters the calcium influx into cells (Zhou et al. 2005), which can have far reaching effects on other calcium-dependent processes, such as gene regulation, synaptic plasticity, or protein expression. The learning impairment seen in this study might be due to secondary effects on other potassium currents or calcium-dependent processes.This study indicates that normal activity of BK channels is required for acquisition of the tEBC task. The channel may act to maintain neurons within a narrow window of excitability, keeping neurons within an operating range of “optimal excitability.” BK channel activity is strongly modulated by kinase-phosphotase activity (Reinhart et al. 1991; Loane et al. 2006), and reduction of BK-mediated current through modulators of these molecules may have a more beneficial impact on learning. Since blocking of the BK channel with paxilline impedes learning, this drug may not present a useful tool for pharmacological learning-enhancement manipulations.  相似文献   

15.
Two experiments examined the neural mechanisms underlying the ontogenetic emergence of auditory eyeblink conditioning. Previous studies found that the medial auditory thalamus is necessary for eyeblink conditioning with an auditory conditioned stimulus (CS) in adult rats. In experiment 1, stimulation of the medial auditory thalamus was used as a CS in rat pups trained on postnatal days (P) 17-18, 24-25, or 31-32. All three age groups showed significant acquisition relative to unpaired controls. However, there was an age-related increase in the rate of conditioning. Experiment 2 examined the effect of inactivating the medial auditory thalamus with muscimol on auditory eyeblink conditioning in rats trained on P17-18, 24-25, or 31-32. Rat pups trained on P24-25 and P31-32, but not P17-18, showed a significant reduction in conditioned responses following muscimol infusions. The findings suggest that the thalamic contribution to auditory eyeblink conditioning continues to develop through the first postnatal month.  相似文献   

16.
It has been proposed that the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) is not necessary for delay eyeblink conditioning (DEC). Here, we investigated the involvement of the mPFC in DEC with a soft or loud tone as the conditioned stimulus (CS) by using electrolytic lesions or muscimol inactivation of guinea pig mPFC. Interestingly, when a soft tone was used as a CS, electrolytic lesions of the mPFC significantly retarded acquisition of the conditioned response (CR), and muscimol infusions into mPFC distinctly inhibited the acquisition and expression of CR, but had no significant effect on consolidation of well-learned CR. In contrast, both electrolytic lesions and muscimol inactivation of mPFC produced no significant deficits in the CR when a loud tone was used as the CS, or in the unconditioned response (UR) when a soft or loud tone was used as the CS. These results demonstrate that the mPFC is essential for the DEC with the soft tone CS but not for the DEC with the loud tone CS.  相似文献   

17.
Although past research has highlighted the involvement of limbic structures such as the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and hippocampus in learning, few have addressed the nature of their interaction. The current study of rabbit jaw movement conditioning used a combination of reversible lesions and electrophysiology to examine the involvement of the hippocampus and the ACC during acquisition, performance, and extinction. We found that microinfusions of procaine into the ACC did not significantly alter the rate of behavioral learning or the amplitude of hippocampal conditioned unit responses, but that they disrupted the rhythmic periodicity of conditioned jaw movements. During extinction, whereas controls showed a rapid decline in behavioral CRs and active inhibition of hippocampal unit responses, ACC lesioned rabbits showed a persistence of conditioning-related hippocampal activity and behavioral responding. The results show that the ACC can be important for adaptive suppression of conditioned behavior and suggest a crucial physiological modulation of hippocampus by ACC during extinction.  相似文献   

18.
The interrelationships of cerebellar and cerebral neural circuits in the eyeblink paradigm were explored with the controlled application of cortical spreading depression (CSD) and lidocaine in the New Zealand albino rabbit. The initial research focus was directed toward the involvement of the motor cortex in the conditioned eyeblink response. However, CSD timing and triangulation results indicate that other areas in the cerebral cortex, particularly the auditory cortex (acoustic conditioned stimulus), appear to be critical for the CSD effect on the eyeblink response. In summary: (1) CSD can be elicited, monitored, and timed and its side effects controlled in 97% of awake rabbits in the right and/or left cerebral hemisphere(s) during eyeblink conditioning. (2) The motor cortex appears to play little or no part in classical conditioning of the eyeblink in the rabbit in the delay paradigm. (3) Inactivating the auditory cortex with CSD or lidocaine temporarily impairs the conditioned response during the first 5 to 15 days of training, but has little effect past that point.  相似文献   

19.
Pontine neuronal activation during auditory stimuli increases ontogenetically between postnatal days (P) P17 and P24 in rats. Pontine neurons are an essential component of the conditioned stimulus (CS) pathway for eyeblink conditioning, providing mossy fiber input to the cerebellum. Here we examined whether the developmental limitation in pontine responsiveness to a CS in P17 rats could be overcome by direct stimulation of the CS pathway. Eyeblink conditioning was established in infant rats on P17-P18 and P24-P25 using pontine stimulation as a CS. There were no significant age-related differences in the rate or level of conditioning. Eyeblink conditioned responses established with the stimulation CS were abolished by inactivation of the ipsilateral cerebellar nuclei and overlying cortex in both age groups. The findings suggest that developmental changes in the CS pathway play an important role in the ontogeny of eyeblink conditioning.  相似文献   

20.
Cholinergic systems are critical to the neural mechanisms mediating learning. Reduced nicotinic cholinergic receptor (nAChR) binding is a hallmark of normal aging. These reductions are markedly more severe in some dementias, such as Alzheimer's disease. Pharmacological central nervous system therapies are a means to ameliorate the cognitive deficits associated with normal aging and aging-related dementias. Trace eyeblink conditioning (EBC), a hippocampus- and forebrain-dependent learning paradigm, is impaired in both aged rabbits and aged humans, attributable in part to cholinergic dysfunction. In the present study, we examined the effects of galantamine (3 mg/kg), a cholinesterase inhibitor and nAChR allosteric potentiating ligand, on the acquisition of trace EBC in aged (30–33 months) and young (2–3 months) female rabbits. Trace EBC involves the association of a conditioned stimulus (CS) with an unconditioned stimulus (US), separated by a stimulus-free trace interval. Repeated CS–US pairings results in the development of the conditioned eyeblink response (CR) prior to US onset. Aged rabbits receiving daily injections of galantamine (Aged/Gal) exhibited significant improvements compared with age-matched controls in trials to eight CRs in 10 trial block criterion (P = 0.0402) as well as performance across 20 d of training [F(1,21) = 5.114, P = 0.0345]. Mean onset and peak latency of CRs exhibited by Aged/Gal rabbits also differed significantly [F(1,21) = 6.120/6.582, P = 0.0220/0.0180, respectively] compared with age-matched controls, resembling more closely CR timing of young drug and control rabbits. Galantamine did not improve acquisition rates in young rabbits compared with age-matched controls. These data indicate that by enhancing nicotinic and muscarinic transmission, galantamine is effective in offsetting the learning deficits associated with decreased cholinergic transmission in the aging brain.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号