共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
When animals show both frequent innovation and fast social learning, new behaviours can spread more rapidly through populations
and potentially increase rates of natural selection and speciation, as proposed by A.C. Wilson in his behavioural drive hypothesis.
Comparative work on primates suggests that more innovative species also show more social learning. In this study, we look
at intra-specific variation in innovation and social learning in captive wild-caught pigeons. Performances on an innovative
problem-solving task and a social learning task are positively correlated in 42 individuals. The correlation remains significant
when the effects of neophobia on the two abilities are removed. Neither sex nor dominance rank are associated with performance
on the two tasks. Free-flying flocks of urban pigeons are able to solve the innovative food-finding problem used on captive
birds, demonstrating it is within the range of their natural capacities. Taken together with the comparative literature, the
positive correlation between innovation and social learning suggests that the two abilities are not traded-off. 相似文献
2.
The ability to recognize self has been known to be limited to some animal species, but previous research has focused almost exclusively on the animal's reaction to a mirror. Recent studies suggest that the temporal contingency between a subject's action and the corresponding visual scene reflected in a mirror plays an important role in self-recognition. To assess the roles of visual-proprioceptive contiguity in self-recognition, we explored whether pigeons are able to discriminate videos of themselves with various temporal properties. We trained five pigeons to respond to live video images of themselves (live self-movies) and not to video filmed during previous training sessions (pre-recorded self-movies). Pigeons learned to peck trial-unique live self-movies more frequently than pre-recorded self-movies. We conducted two generalization tests after pigeons learned to discriminate between the two conditions. First, discrimination acquired during training sessions was transferred to a test session involving live self-movies and new pre-recorded self-movies. Second, the same pigeons were tested in extinction procedure using delayed live self-movies and new pre-recorded self-movies. Although pigeons responded to delayed presentations of live self-movies more frequently than to new pre-recorded self-movies, the relative response rate to delayed presentation of live self-movies gradually decreased as the temporal discrepancy between pigeons' own behavior and the corresponding video increased. These results indicate that pigeons' discrimination of self-movies with various temporal properties was based on the temporal contiguity between their behavior and its visual feedback. The methodology used in the present experiment is an important step toward improving the experimental analysis of self-recognition in non-human animals. 相似文献
3.
Time-space learning reflects an ability to represent in memory event-stimulus properties together with the place and time
of the event; a capacity well developed in birds. Homing pigeons were trained in an indoor octagonal arena to locate one food
goal in the morning and a different food goal in the late afternoon. The goals differed with respect to their angular/directional
relationship to an artificial light source located outside the arena. Further, the angular difference in reward position approximated
the displacement of the sun's azimuth that would occur during the same time period. The experimental birds quickly learned
the task, demonstrating the apparent ease with which birds can adopt an artificial light source to discriminate among alternative
spatial responses at different times of the day. However, a novel midday probe session following successful learning revealed
that the light source was interpreted as a stable landmark and not as a surrogate sun that would support compass orientation.
Probe sessions following a phase shift of the light–dark cycle revealed that the mechanism employed to make the temporal discrimination
was prevailingly based on an endogenous circadian rhythm and not an interval timing mechanism. 相似文献
4.
Lombardi CM 《Animal cognition》2008,11(1):67-74
Relational learning, as opposed to perceptual learning, is based on the abstract properties of the stimuli. Although at present
there is no doubt that pigeons are capable of relational behavior, this study aims to further disclose the conditions under
which it occurs. Pigeons were trained in an outdoor cage on a matching-to-sample or an oddity-from-sample task, with colored
cardboard stimuli presented horizontally. The apparatus involved three sliding lids on which the stimuli were drawn and which,
when displaced, revealed the reinforcement. The lids were either adjacent to each other or somewhat separated. Training sessions
involved two colors, and test sessions six different colors (same dimension test), or six different shapes (different dimension
test). One group of birds trained under the ‘adjacent’ condition failed when tested with new stimuli, but succeeded in both
dimension tests after training under the ‘separate’ condition. Two other groups of birds succeeded in all tests after training
under the latter condition. These results show that depending on procedural details, pigeons are or are not able to transfer
from one visual dimension to another, thus extending previous related findings. 相似文献
5.
Change blindness is a phenomenon whereby changes to a stimulus are more likely go unnoticed under certain circumstances. Pigeons learned a change detection task, in which they observed sequential stimulus displays consisting of individual colors back-projected onto three response keys. The color of one response key changed during each sequence and pecks to the key that displayed the change were reinforced. Pigeons showed a change blindness effect, in that change detection accuracy was worse when there was an inter-stimulus interval interrupting the transition between consecutive stimulus displays. Birds successfully transferred to stimulus displays involving novel colors, indicating that pigeons learned a general change detection rule. Furthermore, analysis of responses to specific color combinations showed that pigeons could detect changes involving both spectral and non-spectral colors and that accuracy was better for changes involving greater differences in wavelength. These results build upon previous investigations of change blindness in both humans and pigeons and suggest that change blindness may be a general consequence of selective visual attention relevant to multiple species and stimulus dimensions. 相似文献
6.
Ming Xu 《Journal of Philosophical Logic》2015,44(6):851-877
This paper presents a short survey of recent developments in stit theories, with an emphasis on combinations of stit and deontic logic, and those of stit and epistemic logic. 相似文献
7.
In Daoist philosophy, the self is understood as an individual interdependent with others, and situated within a broader environment.
Within this framework, the concept ziran is frequently understood in terms of naturalness or nature while wuwei is explained in terms of non-oppressive government. In many existing accounts, little is done to connect these two key Daoist
concepts. Here, I suggest that wuwei and ziran are correlated, ethical, concepts. Together, they provide a unifying ethical framework for understanding the philosophy of the Daodejing. I explore the meaning of ziran as self-so-ness or, in human terms, as pertaining to an individual’s spontaneity. The appropriate response to the spontaneity
of individuals is to avoid, insofar as possible, imposing or using restrictive norms and methods, that is, wuwei. According to this view, ziran and wuwei offer an account of ethics that attends to core notions of interdependent selfhood, including mutuality, relationality, interdependence,
symbiosis, and responsiveness. 相似文献
8.
Pigeons were trained to locate food in two geometrically equivalent corners of a parallelogram-shaped enclosure. Both the
angular amplitude of the corners and the length of the walls alone were sufficient for successfully completing the task. Following
training, birds were tested in three separate conditions that manipulated the geometric information available. During tests
in both a rectangular-shaped enclosure that preserved the wall length information but not the angular amplitude, and a rhombus-shaped
enclosure that did the opposite, pigeons located their goal corners with a high degree of accuracy, indicating an ability
to use both types of geometric information in isolation. This result is consistent with prior research with domestic chicks.
However, in a conflict test in a reverse parallelogram-shaped enclosure, in which the correct angular location was paired
with an incorrect wall length location, birds showed a preference for the correct angular location. This suggests that pigeons
weight angles more heavily than wall lengths in this type of navigation task, which differs from findings in a similar task
conducted with the domestic chick. Results in the conflict test also suggest that pigeons did not use the principal axis as
their main strategy of small-scale navigation. 相似文献
9.
Carsten Held 《Journal for General Philosophy of Science》2017,48(3):439-452
Antecedent-strengthening, a trivially valid inference of classical logic of the form: P → Q ? (P & R) → Q, has a counterpart in everyday reasoning that often fails. A plausible solution to the problem involves assuming an implicit ceteris paribus (CP) qualifier that can be explicated as an additional conjunct in the antecedent of the premise. The qualifier can be explicated as ‘everything else relevant remains unchanged’ or alternatively as ‘nothing interferes’. The qualifier appears most prominently in the context of the discussion of laws in the sciences, where these laws are often expressed with a CP qualifier. From an analysis of the qualifier’s role in the problem of antecedent-strengthening, we can learn more about CP qualifiers in general and in their application to the laws used in the sciences. 相似文献
10.
Luis R. G. Oliveira 《Philosophical Studies》2018,175(10):2567-2582
Deontological evidentialism is the claim that S ought to form or maintain S’s beliefs in accordance with S’s evidence. A promising argument for this view turns on the premise that consideration c is a normative reason for S to form or maintain a belief that p only if c is evidence that p is true. In this paper, I discuss the surprising relation between a recently influential argument for this key premise and the principle that ought implies can. I argue that anyone who antecedently accepts or rejects this principle already has a reason to resist either this argument’s premises or its role in support of deontological evidentialism. 相似文献
11.
12.
Miller FC 《American journal of psychoanalysis》2011,71(2):94-109
Five important transitional tasks of adolescent development are (i) taming the upsurge of desires and impulses, both sexual and aggressive, into constructive and creative directions; (ii) establishing independence from infantile family ties (while maintaining some involvement with the family of origin); (iii) reconciling self-preoccupations with social attachments; (iv) reworking identifications, especially sexual; and (v) establishing romantic attachments and solidifying ongoing stable love relationships. These tasks are illustrated with the help of three movies, namely Ordinary People, Fly Away Home, and (500) Days of Summer. 相似文献
13.
Kamlesh Singh Pulkit Khanna Meenakshi Khosla Mounica Rapelly Akarsh Soni 《Journal of religion and health》2018,57(4):1392-1401
Sat-Chit-Ananda is an indigenous construct that refers to absolute bliss and consciousness. The present study aimed to strengthen the psychometric properties of the newly developed Sat-Chit-Ananda scale (Singh et al. in Int J Vedic Found Manag 1(2):54–74, 2014). A total of 398 students aged 17–36 years (mean age = 21.33 years, SD = 2.2, 70% males) participated in this study. An exploratory as well as confirmatory factor analysis was computed for the 17-item scale. Its’ concurrent validity was established by assessing its correlation with other well-being measures, namely Flourishing (Diener et al. in Soc Indic Res 97:143–156 2010) and Scale of Positive and Negative Experience (Diener et al. 2010). Satisfactory results were obtained from both exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses. Sat-Chit-Ananda and its factors were found to be significantly positively correlated with Flourishing and Positive Experiences and were negatively correlated with Negative Experiences. Thus, the validity of the Sat-Chit-Ananda (Singh et al. 2014) scale stands further substantiated—offering this scale as a promising new assessment tool. 相似文献
14.
Yohan Ka 《Pastoral Psychology》2010,59(2):221-231
This article explores a Korean culture-bound emotion called jeong-han, which shares similarities with narcissism and depression in terms of causes, formation processes, emotions, cognitions,
and neurotic sufferings. This article has three main goals: 1) to report and describe the presence and experience of jeong-han among contemporary Koreans; 2) to compare jeong-han with narcissism and depression; and 3) to suggest an effective way of dealing with jeong-han through jeong-dynamics. In order to effectively help people with jeong-han both in counseling and ministry settings, providing jeong and facilitating jeong-dynamics in a small group or community setting can be helpful. 相似文献
15.
16.
Enrique Huelva Unternbäumen 《Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences》2017,16(1):107-131
The principal aim of this paper is to analyze the relationship between intersubjectivity and grammar. We argue that intersubjectivity represents, on the one hand, a prerequisite for the development of language as a symbolic system, and therefore also for the development of grammar. Furthermore, we attempt to show that language, and especially grammar, codify intersubjectivity. That is to say, grammatical constructions represent the intersubjective interactions that situated agents maintain in different pragmatic contects. We call this phenomenon the meta-representational capacity of language. Our main object of analysis is the development of the ditransitive construction (give something to someone - dar algo a alguien) in the Spanish language. The evolution of this construction makes it clear that there is an important correlation between the degree of complexity of the codified intersubjective interaction and grammatically obligatory nature and the prominence of the grammatical construction that codifies it: the greater the complexity, the greater its obligatoriness, and vice versa. 相似文献
17.
18.
Francesca Boccuni 《Studia Logica》2010,96(2):315-330
PG (Plural Grundgesetze) is a predicative monadic second-order system which exploits the notion of plural quantification and a few Fregean devices,
among which a formulation of the infamous Basic Law V. It is shown that second-order Peano arithmetic can be derived in PG.
I also investigate the philosophical issue of predicativism connected to PG. In particular, as predicativism about concepts
seems rather un-Fregean, I analyse whether there is a way to make predicativism compatible with Frege’s logicism. 相似文献
19.
20.
Scientific writings and policy documents define the terms nanomaterial and nanoparticle in various ways. This variation is considered problematic because the absence of a shared definition is understood as potentially hindering nanomaterial knowledge production and regulation. Another view is that the existence of a shared definition may itself cause problems, as rigid definitions arguably exclude important aspects of the studied phenomena. The aim of this paper is to inform this state of disagreement by providing analytical concepts for a systematic understanding of how, and even whether, nanomaterial and nanoparticle could and should be defined. To do this, we review definitions of nanomaterial and nanoparticle presented in research articles and policy documents. Definitions were identified by first conducting a Scopus search and then tracing cited definitions back to their sources. In total, 36 definitions were identified. Theories of definition from philosophy and linguistics provide analytical guidance for structuring and categorizing the identified definitions, and the main analytical dimensions of the definitions are then identified and discussed. Finally, we propose a framework for understanding the process of defining nanomaterial and nanoparticle. This framework considers both the generality needed for a shared understanding (by suggesting proto-definitions of nanomaterial and nanoparticle) and the level of precision required for different purposes (by allowing for various explications of the proto-definitions). 相似文献