首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 31 毫秒
1.
从发展心理学的角度.对幼儿到大学六个年龄组的儿童青少年进行了事件相关电位P_(300)测定.结果发现:反映大脑认知功能的事件相关电位P_(300)各成分随着年龄增长而发展的趋势十分明显,从而进一步说明了认知发展的特点与大脑高级神经系统活动功能发展特点的密切关系。  相似文献   

2.
Person–organization (P–O) fit was examined as an antecedent of individuals’ attraction to organizations by operationalizing P–O fit as the similarity between individuals and organizations on three points of comparison: personality dimensions, values, and goals. It was hypothesized that compared to P–O fit on values and on goals, P–O fit on personality dimensions would be related more strongly to organizational attractiveness. It was also hypothesized that relative to P–O fit on goals, P–O fit on values would be related more strongly to organizational attractiveness. The results indicated that each of the points of comparison had a unique effect on organizational attractiveness and that individuals were more attracted to organizations that were similar to them than to organizations that were dissimilar to them.  相似文献   

3.
In a temporally defined system of reinforcement schedules, the fixed interval case is defined when reinforcement probability, P, is equal to unity for the first response in any cycle length, T; when P is less than 1.0, random interval schedules emerge wherein T/P specifies the expected interval between reinforcements. Key-pecking rates were found to be: (a) inversely related to T/P; (b) higher at T=1.0 second than at other T parameter values; (c) low and linear at several T and T/P values. The mean post-reinforcement pause, if initially small, increased, and if initially large, decreased, as T/P increased.  相似文献   

4.
When stretching muscles of the ankle joint, stretch velocity and amplitude were shown to selectively influence specific parts of the stretch reflex. The present study investigated whether similar effects can be observed at the knee joint. Seventeen subjects were exposed to sudden anterior tibial translations. The influence of stimulus amplitude was analyzed by applying a low (LIMP) or high impulse (HIMP). To investigate the influence of velocity, rate of force development of the perturbation was chosen either low (LRFD) or high (HRFD). Activation of biceps femoris (BF), semitendinosus (ST), vastus medialis (VM) and vastus lateralis (VL) was calculated in four consecutive timeframes (P0, P1, P2, P3). During P1, RFD (ST: p < .05; BF: p < .01; VM: p < .05; VL: p > .05) and during P2, impulse (ST: p < .05; BF: p < .01; VM: p < .01; VL: p < .01) did significantly influence reflex activation. The present study showed that stimulus characteristics influenced specific reflex components of knee joint muscles. As only hamstring muscles were stretched, whereas quadriceps was unloaded, it is concluded that different mechanisms like homonymous and heteronymous muscle afferents as well as joint and skin afferents might contribute to the reflex responses.  相似文献   

5.
A constellation of joint probability estimates is semantically coherent when the quantitative relationship among estimates of P(A), P(B), P(A and B), and P(A or B) is consistent with the relationship among the sets described in the problem statement. The possible probability estimates can form an extremely large number of permutations. However, this entire problem space can be reduced to six theoretically meaningful patterns: logically fallacious (conjunction or disjunction fallacies), identical sets (e.g., water and H2O), mutually exclusive sets (e.g., horses and zebras), subsets (e.g., robins and birds), overlapping sets (e.g., accountants and musicians), and inconsistent overlapping sets. Determining which of these patterns describes any set of probability estimates has been automated using Excel spreadsheet formulae. Researchers may use the semantic coherence technique to examine the consequences of differently worded problems, individual differences, or experimental manipulations. The spreadsheet described above can be downloaded as a supplement from http://brm.psychonomic-journals.org/content/supplemental.  相似文献   

6.
Reduced amplitude of the P300 event-related potential has been consistently associated with a variety of externalising problems, including conduct disorder. The few available genetically-informative studies of these relationships, however, were conducted among adolescents/adults (i.e., at an age when conduct disorder has typically already become manifest). Among 200 general population twins with a mean age of 9 years (range 6–14 years), we studied the relationship between the P300 waveform elicited by an auditory oddball task and the DSM-oriented conduct problems scale of the Child Behavior Checklist 6–18. Conduct problems scores were negatively and significantly correlated (r?=??0.19, p?=?0.01) with P300 amplitude; correlations between P300 amplitude and the other DSM-oriented Child Behavior Checklist scales were non-significant, except for oppositional defiant problems (p?=?0.01). We found moderate heritability estimates for both P300 amplitude (0.58, CI:0.37;0.73) and conduct problems (0.52, CI:0.25;0.70). Bivariate twin analyses indicated that the covariation between these two phenotypes can be explained by additive genetic factors only, with a genetic correlation of ?0.33. An association between reduced P300 amplitude and conduct problems can be substantiated already in childhood, at an age that precedes the most typical onset of conduct disorder. This relationship appears to be genetic in nature. Reduced P300 amplitude can represent a valuable marker for conduct problems, and can contribute to the early identification of children at high-risk for conduct disorder.  相似文献   

7.
Abstract

Richard Swinburne, in his The Existence of God (2004), presents a cosmological argument in defence of theism (Swinburne 1991: 119, 135). God, Swinburne argues, is more likely to bring about an ordered universe than other states (ibid.: 144, 299). To defend this view, Swinburne presents the following arguments: (1) That this ordered universe is a priori improbable (2004: 49, 150, 1991: 304 et seq.), given the stringent requirements for life (cf. also Leslie 2000: 12), and the Second Law of Thermodynamics (Giancoli 1990: 396); (2) That it seems as if this ordered universe can be explained by theism; (3) A theistic explanation for the universe is more probable because it is a simple explanation. To this end, Swinburne makes use of Bayes’ Theorem. Symbolically, this claim can be represented as (e) for the evidence of the existence of a complex universe, and (h) for a hypothesis. Swinburne’s argument is that theism has a higher prior probability, P(htheism) > P(hmaterialism), since theism is simpler than materialism. He concludes that P(e|htheism) > P(e|hmaterialism). In this paper I will address only this argument (3) above, and defend the view that it is false: theism is not simpler than materialism, nor it is more probably true. I conclude that theism is less probable than materialism, expressed by P(htheism) < P(hmaterialism) : 2/N(2n+1) < 1/n, where N is the number of possible universes and n the number of entities in existence.  相似文献   

8.
采用Flanker范式,通过事件相关电位(ERPs)技术,探讨预期背离情境下的脑电反应及与敏感性个体的关系。选取17名被试,采取2×2组内设计刺激类型(奖励vs.惩罚)和预期受挫(是vs.否);结果表明(1)预期受挫情境引发更高的FRN;(2)惩罚刺激引发更高的FRN,奖励刺激引发更高的P3;(3)惩罚敏感与惩罚、无奖励情境引发FRN相关。本实验结果表明(1)FRN既反应对反馈效价的加工,也可能是预期背离的一种表现;(2)无奖励刺激受惩罚系统调节;无惩罚刺激受奖励系统调节。  相似文献   

9.
In an attempt to elucidate the nature of the subject’s strategy in a two-interval forcedchoice auditory detection task, event-related potentials were studied at two intensities which yielded mean accuracies of 82% and 98%. Subjects reported the observation interval in which they judged the signal to be present and the confidence of the judgment. Principal components varimax analyses yielded four components: a CZ maximal P300, a Slow Wave, a slow negative shift, and a late negative component. The P300 amplitude findings suggest that different strategies are utilized for high-confidence and low-confidence detections. At high confidence, P300 amplitude is large for the observation interval in which the signal is presented, indicating a strategy involving serial independent detection. However, the P300 latency findings at high confidence suggest that absence of the signal in the first observation interval is nonetheless noted: P300 latency in response to signal presence is shorter for the second observation interval than for the first observation interval. At low confidence, P300 is small or absent for both observation intervals, indicating a deferred decision, presumably arrived at through comparison of the two percepts.  相似文献   

10.
The cinematograms of 12 two-state elements arranged in the clock positions in space and in a sequence of adjacent 100-ms frames in time were used as stimuli. Some positions in each frame (or all 12 of them) could be labeled as "domain" ones, and every element that was T frames and S positions (clockwise or counterclockwise) apart from a domain element could repeat the latter's state with probability P. The probability of the rotation direction identification was obtained as a function of T, S, P, number of frames, and the domain positions selection scheme. A generalized version of the reversed phi phenomenon was obtained: if P less than .5, then the psychometric value lies below .5 level. All the data can be accounted for by a simple model according to which the choice of direction is based on the counts of the different types of dipoles, each type being characterized by the probability and the weight of its count: In most situations all dipoles but the shortest ones (connecting the neighboring elements of successive frames) can be ignored.  相似文献   

11.
A two-interval detection procedure was used to study the effects of signal probability on the listening band. The signal was chosen randomly on each trial from a set of three sinusoid segments of 650, 750, or 850 Hz. The relative likelihood of a specific frequency signal being present on a trial was varied over three experimental conditions: (I) P(650) = P(750) = P(850) = .33; (2) P(650) = P(850) = .17, P(750) =.66; and (3) P(750) = 1.0. Plots of P(C) vs signal frequency indicated small but consistent differences between the three experimental conditions. The effective listening band appears to be sharper when the center frequency signal has a higher presentation probability. Analysis of the sequential probabilities failed to support an interpretation of the results in terms of short-term band changes. It is argued that there are relatively long term changes in the effective listening band as a function of presentation probability.  相似文献   

12.
Reviews     
《Zygon》1983,18(2):189-206
Books reviewed in this article.
Genes, Mind and Culture: The Coevolutionary Process. By C harles J. L umsden and E dward O. W ilson .
Myth, Creativity, Psychoanalysis: Essays in Honor of Harry Slochower. Edited by M aynard S olomon .
The Mind-Body Problem: A Psychobiological Approach. By M ario B unge .
Towards a New Mysticism: Teihard de Chardin and Eastern Religions. By U rsula K ing .
The Practice of Process Meditation: The Intensive Journal Way to Spiritual Experience. By I ra P rocoff .
Meaning and the Moral Sciences. By H ilary P utnam .
Faith, Science and the Future. Edited by P aul A brecht .
Faith and Science in an Unjust World: Report of the World Council of Churches' Conference on Faith, Science and the Future , vol. 1: Plenary Presentations. Edited by R oger L. S hinn .
Faith and Science in an Unjust World , vol. 2: Reports and Recommendations. Edited by P aul A brecht .
The Image of Eternity: Roots of Time in the Physical World. By D avid P ark .  相似文献   

13.
《Personnel Psychology》1979,32(4):767-767
  相似文献   

14.
Taylor and Klein (Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 26:1639–1656, 2000) discovered two mutually exclusive “flavors” of inhibition of return (IOR): When the oculomotor system is “actively suppressed,” IOR affects input processes (the perception/attention flavor), whereas when the oculomotor system is “engaged,” IOR affects output processes (the motor flavor). Studies of brain activity with ignored cues have typically reported that IOR reduces an early sensory event-related potential (ERP) component (i.e., the P1 component) of the brain’s response to the target. Since eye movements were discouraged in these experiments, the P1 reduction might be a reflection of the perception/attention flavor of IOR. If, instead of ignoring the cue, participants made a prosaccade to the cue (and then returned to fixation) before responding to the target, the motor flavor of IOR should then be generated. We compared these two conditions while monitoring eye position and recording ERPs to the targets. If the P1 modulation is related to the perceptual/attentional flavor of IOR, we hypothesized that it might be absent when the motoric flavor of IOR was generated by a prosaccade to the cue. Our results demonstrated that target-related P1 reductions and behavioral IOR were similar, and significant, in both conditions. However, P1 modulations were significantly correlated with behavioral IOR only when the oculomotor system was actively suppressed, suggesting that P1 modulations may only affect behaviorally exhibited IOR when the attentional/perceptual flavor of IOR is recruited.  相似文献   

15.
Book Reviews     
《Political psychology》1998,19(4):853-878
Sudhir Kakar, The Colors of Violence: Cultural Identities, Religion, and Conflict
Paul Kivel Uprooting Racism Benjamin P. Bowser and Raymond G. Hunt (eds.), Impacts of Racism on White Americans, Second Edition
Michael S. Lund, Preventing Violent Conflicts: A Strategy for Preventive Diplomacy
Marc Howard Ross, The Management of Conflict: Interpretations and Interests in Comparative Perspective
Amos Kiewe (ed.), The Modern Presidency and Crisis Rhetoric
Sarah Sanderson King and Donald P. Cushman (eds.), Political Communication: Engineering Visions of Order in the Socialist World
Donald P. Green and Ian Shapiro, Pathologies of Rational Choice  相似文献   

16.
刘志雅  莫雷 《心理学报》2011,43(1):92-100
采用学习-迁移模式, 探讨了同时学习和继时学习两种方式下归类不确定时的特征推理。共包括2个实验, 其中实验1探讨了固定学习轮次的情况, 实验2探讨了固定学习正确率的情况。实验结果表明:同时呈现类别要素的同时学习方式下, 被试习得序列式的单类别表征(原型表征), 在归类不确定时的特征推理中按照“单类的Bayesian规则”进行特征推理, 即P(j\F) =P(k\F)·P(j\k); 相继呈现类别要素的继时学习方式下, 被试习得并列式的多类别表征, 在归类不确定时的特征推理中按照“理性模型”进行推理, 即 P(j\F) =Σk P(k\F)·P(j\k)。  相似文献   

17.
Reviews     
《新多明我会修道士》1980,61(726):542-543
Book reviewed in this article:
Understanding Mysticism edited by; Richard woods O P imaga book Double-day
Reflections On The Beatitudes: Soundings in Christian Traditions, by simon tugwall O.P.  相似文献   

18.
This study tested the hypothesis that the moderate correlation between optimism (O) and pessimism (P) scores (their non-bipolarity) found in earlier studies may be an artifact of the differential sensitivity of O and P items to two response sets: defensive pessimism and Pollyannaism. The data failed to support either component of the hypothesis. Thus, defensive pessimists, as measured in two quite different ways, had lower depression scores (BDI) than genuine pessimists, but contrary to prediction, had higher depression scores than optimists. Moreover, deleting P items with a strong defensive quality from the P scale failed to increase the correlation between O and P scores. Similarly, controlling Pollyannaism by a partial correlation procedure failed to increase the strength of the relation between O and P. The results in general support the notion that optimism and pessimism are not polar opposites. This article is based on a dissertation presented by the senior author to the Division of Graduate Studies and Research of the University of Cincinnati in partial fulfillment of the Doctor of Philosophy degree.  相似文献   

19.
刘雁伶  胡竹菁 《心理科学》2013,36(3):716-721
摘 要:使用纸笔测验探讨表格集中呈现信息条件下取样大小对单一因果关系强度推理的影响,并比较五种模型ΔP、效力PC、SS效力、Support和χ2的预测与实验数据的相关。结果显示:(1)取样大小对不同的问题有不同的影响:高取样在ΔP=0时导致了低估计值,在0<|ΔP|=PPC时没有效果,在|ΔP|相似文献   

20.
This study tested the hypothesis that the moderate correlation between optimism (O) and pessimism (P) scores (their non-bipolarity) found in earlier studies may be an artifact of the differential sensitivity of O and P items to two response sets: defensive pessimism and Pollyannaism. The data failed to support either component of the hypothesis. Thus, defensive pessimists, as measured in two quite different ways, had lower depression scores (BDI) than genuine pessimists, but contrary to prediction, had higher depression scores than optimists. Moreover, deleting P items with a strong defensive quality from the P scale failed to increase the correlation between O and P scores. Similarly, controlling Pollyannaism by a partial correlation procedure failed to increase the strength of the relation between O and P. The results in general support the notion that optimism and pessimism are not polar opposites. This article is based on a dissertation presented by the senior author to the Division of Graduate Studies and Research of the University of Cincinnati in partial fulfillment of the Doctor of Philosophy degree.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号