首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 890 毫秒
1.
创设了习俗领域和个人领域两类道义情境故事,在每类故事中又呈现领域内冲突和领域间冲突两种规则冲突类型,考察了176名中学生对道义情境故事中行为者和规则制定者的想法、感受和行为倾向等方面的认识,来分析中学生心理理论的发展特点。结果表明:(1)中学生被预测对于习俗领域中的两个规则倾向会给予相同的理解,在个人领域中会倾向于接受其他制定者的规则。(2)教师被预测在领域间冲突规则中会对违规学生有较多负面看法。(3)不同年级、不同性别中学生对冲突道义情境中心理理论问题的认识基本一致。结论:中学生对于中学生自身及成人的认识和行为预测,因不同的道义情境领域和冲突规则而会有所不同。  相似文献   

2.
7~15岁儿童对"集体可交流责任"的心理反应特点之研究   总被引:1,自引:1,他引:0  
王丽  岑国桢 《心理科学》2004,27(6):1310-1313
编撰不同内容、背景和投射情境的故事为刺激材料.以173名7-15岁儿童为被试,探究他们在集体可交流责任问题上的认知、情感、承担倾向的反应。结果表明:我国7-15岁儿童也具有Piaget发现的集体可交流责任观;对它的理解接受在7-9岁之间有明显的变化;对它的情感反应均为消极的体验;对它的承担倾向上7、9岁比11、13、15岁儿童更为明显;一般和角色的两种投射情境、不知道犯过者和不揭发犯过者两种故事背景对认知反应、承担倾向反应有明显影响;声誉损害、财物损坏、人身伤害三种内容对认知反应、情感反应会有不同的影响。  相似文献   

3.
创设了习俗领域和个人领域两类道义情境故事,在每类故事中又呈现领域内冲突和领域间冲突两种规则冲突类型,考察了176名中学生对道义情境故事中行为者和规则制定者的想法、感受和行为倾向等方面的认识,来分析中学生心理理论的发展特点。结果表明:(1)中学生被预测对于习俗领域中的两个规则倾向会给予相同的理解,在个人领域中会倾向于接受其他制定者的规则。(2)教师被预测在领域间冲突规则中会对违规学生有较多负面看法。(3)不同年级、不同性别中学生对冲突道义情境中心理理论问题的认识基本一致。结论中学生对于中学生自身及成人的认识和行为预测,因不同的道义情境领域和冲突规则而会有所不同。  相似文献   

4.
6~12岁儿童道德移情、助人行为倾向及其关系的研究   总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3  
岑国桢  王丽  李胜男 《心理科学》2004,27(4):781-785
以124名6~12岁儿童为被试、以情境故事为材料的测查表明:我国6~12岁儿童均能作出移情反应和一般助人行为倾向反应,但8岁以上儿童的反应更为强烈和成熟;8岁以上儿童才能在自己也有困难的冲突背景下仍作出助人行为倾向的反应;6~12岁儿童的移情反应与其一般助人行为倾向反应有显著正相关关系;移情反应与冲突背景下的助人行为倾向反应的显著正相关8岁以上儿童才具有;个人、集体两类情境会影响他们的移情反应;人身伤害、声誉损害、财物损坏三种情境会影响他们的助人行为倾向。  相似文献   

5.
以974名14-18岁的中学生为被试,通过道德情绪的词汇评定和情境评定,考察中学生对典型道德情绪种类和道德情绪典型属性的认识。结果表明,中学生对典型道德情绪的认识涉及情境性、导向性、批评性、赞誉性等多种情绪类别,并在总体上更容易把正性情绪词汇认同为道德情绪。在判断与评价具体情境中的道德情绪过程中,中学生更倾向把无私和有私因素诱发的情绪显著地聚类区分,从而将无私诱因视为其认同道德情绪的典型标准,这种内隐观不受其学段、性别的影响和情绪效价效应的干扰。  相似文献   

6.
假想的道德两难情境下小学儿童对说谎或说真话的抉择   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
傅根跃  王玲凤 《心理科学》2006,29(5):1049-1052
研究探讨了294名7、9、11岁的小学儿童在道德两难情境下对说谎或说真话的道德行为抉择。结果表明,在个人谎故事条件下,随着年龄的增长,儿童越来越倾向于选择说真话;在集体谎故事条件下,11岁儿童比7岁、9岁儿童更倾向于选择说谎。7岁儿童已具有初步的集体观念,随着年龄的增长,小学儿童的集体观念日趋稳定。随着年龄的增长,小学儿童更多地从诚实、为集体或为个人等多个方面来陈述说谎或说真话的理由。  相似文献   

7.
使用中性视频游戏和亲社会视频游戏,采用单次双人给予倾向困境任务和辣酱范式分别测量被试的合作倾向和攻击倾向,旨在探究竞争性视频游戏情境对玩家合作倾向和攻击倾向的影响。结果发现:(1)在对合作倾向的影响上,无论是玩中性视频游戏还是亲社会视频游戏,竞争情境相比单人情境均能显著降低玩家的合作倾向;(2)在对攻击倾向的影响上,玩中性视频游戏,竞争情境相比单人情境能显著提高玩家的攻击倾向;但玩亲社会视频游戏,竞争情境与单人情境对玩家攻击倾向的影响差异不显著。本研究表明,游戏情境是影响玩家行为倾向的重要因素之一,在非暴力视频游戏中竞争情境对玩家行为倾向会产生消极影响,亲社会视频游戏在一定程度上能抑制竞争情境的消极效应。  相似文献   

8.
6~9岁儿童特质观与其人格特质的关系   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
杨丽珠  高雯 《心理科学》2007,30(4):839-843
为考察特质观与稳定的行为方式之间的关系,采用情境故事法和问卷法分别对180名6~9岁儿童进行了特质稳定性理解的个别测查和人格特质的教师评定。结果发现,持三种不同特质观(实体论、中间论、渐变论)的6~9岁儿童开始在认真有恒、自制稳重、聪慧性、开朗活泼和同情利他五种人格特质行为上表现出显著差异;6~9岁儿童的特质观能够预测以上五种人格特质,丽儿童“开朗插泼”和“聪慧性”特质行为的线性组合是预测儿童特质观的最佳指标。这些研究结论有助于说明关于特质的认知在调解和制约个体稳定的行为理解和反应中的重要作用,验证了人格认知流派的观点;支持了认知与人格之间交互作用的理论假设;对于解释年幼儿童发展中的不适应问题具有一定的意义。  相似文献   

9.
采用大学生人际自立量表、人际信任量表、大五人格简式量表等工具对1345名大学生进行的调查和情境研究显示:控制大五人格后,人际开放仍能预测人际信任倾向,并能通过人际信任倾向的中介作用间接预测人际信任的认知和行为反应;人际责任仍能直接预测人际信任的认知与行为反应。这提示,在对人际信任的预测方面,人际责任和人际开放具有大五人格所不能解释的独特作用,并在一定程度上支持了人格与人际信任反应的人际信任倾向中介模型。  相似文献   

10.
以332名新生代农民工为被试,从社会比较的视角,探讨威胁情境下新生代农民工社会比较策略与身份选择的关系。研究采用了虚拟创设威胁情境以及问卷调查的方法,测查了不同认同类型农民工比较策略使用的特点。结果表明:在威胁情境下,不同认同类型的农民工在比较策略的使用上均存在显著差异,其中城市人认同类型的农民工倾向于采用回避比较策略;农村人认同类型的农民工倾向于采用选择新的比较维度策略;模糊认同类型的农民工倾向于采用降低比较维度重要性策略。  相似文献   

11.
Despite increasing public, professional, and scientific interest in the problem of wife abuse and battering, little has been written about the importance of sociocultural sensitivity in intervention with abused and battered women in Arab society. In this article, I describe central family values in that society and discuss their relevance to wife abuse and battering. Specifically, the discussion focuses on values such as mutual family support and interdependence, family reputation, women's inferiority and male supremacy, and family cohesion and the relevance of those values to wife abuse and battering. In addition, I present Arab women's perspectives on this problem, as revealed in their responses to open-ended questions that were part of larger studies conducted by me. In this regard, five dimensions of their perspectives are considered: (1) Women's definitions of violence against women; (2) their awareness of the problem; (3) their justification or condemnation of violence against women; (4) their awareness of the risk encountered by battered women; and (5) their approach toward coping with the problem. The results are discussed from the perspective of the sociocultural context of Arab society. The article concludes with recommendations for socioculturally sensitive intervention with battered women in this society.  相似文献   

12.
Conventionally, when choice is measured under concurrent schedules, all responses are included. However, the class of all responses consists of 2 sub-classes which are discriminable by their properties: Reinforced responses always equal obtained reinforcers, while unreinforced responses are free to vary. As a result, the inclusion of reinforced responses in choice measures results in sensitivity values in generalized matching that are biased toward larger values, and this bias becomes extreme in some combinations of overall response and reinforcer rates. Different ways of varying concurrent-schedule values also affect estimated sensitivity and the linearity between choice based on all responses and reinforcer ratios. To avoid spurious results and comparisons, and as a matter of good practice, generalized-matching fits and measures should be done using only unreinforced responses.  相似文献   

13.
Scales whose categories are labeled with ranges of ratio values are compared with verbal category scales and magnitude estimation. Relative perceived intensities of Eugenol odor were scaled by power law methods, using 102 Ss, five scaling methods-one verbal, two numerical, and two magnitude estimation-and making comparisons against two alternate odor reference standards. Variations in the psychophysical exponent values derived under each condition were examined. Comparisons between scale types were made: numerical ratio-range category scales may behave as magnitude estimations or as category scales depending on the way responses are scored by the experimenter.  相似文献   

14.
The effects of discussion on subsequent group and individual choices are studied in a situation where subjects choose between a sure gain of varying amount and five probability levels associated with larger gains of expected value equal to that of the sure gain. At the end of the experiment, a single bet, chosen at random, is played for money. Before discussion, subjects have to guess the percentage of similar, more risky and more cautious choices made by their peers for each of the six bets. As predicted by a majority-rule decision-making model significant risky shifts were observed for relatively low values of sure gain. For higher values, however, groups tended to be more cautious than individuals. The final private choices of individuals were significantly more risky than their initial decisions. Most individuals apparently thought they were at least as risky as most others. This finding was due, however, primarily to the responses of subjects who chose the highest risk-level (the ceiling effect) and, secondly, to the consistent tendency of most individuals to guess that others make the same choices as they themselves. It is concluded that majority influence seems a satisfactory explanation of group risky shifts observed in the present study, but it cannot account for modifications of group and individual choices in all risk-taking situations.  相似文献   

15.
We tested whether listeners are differentially responsive to the presence or absence of voicing, a salient, distinguishing acoustic feature, in laughter. Each of 128 participants rated 50 voiced and 20 unvoiced laughs twice according to one of five different rating strategies. Results were highly consistent regardless of whether participants rated their own emotional responses, likely responses of other people, or one of three perceived attributes concerning the laughers, thus indicating that participants were experiencing similarly differentiated affective responses in all these cases. Specifically, voiced, songlike laughs were significantly more likely to elicit positive responses than were variants such as unvoiced grunts, pants, and snortlike sounds. Participants were also highly consistent in their relative dislike of these other sounds, especially those produced by females. Based on these results, we argue that laughers use the acoustic features of their vocalizations to shape listener affect.  相似文献   

16.
Attempts to characterize voluntary behavior have been ongoing for thousands of years. We provide experimental evidence that judgments of volition are based upon distributions of responses in relation to obtained rewards. Participants watched as responses, said to be made by "actors," appeared on a computer screen. The participant's task was to estimate how well each actor represented the voluntary choices emitted by a real person. In actuality, all actors' responses were generated by algorithms based on Baum's (1979) generalized matching function. We systematically varied the exponent values (sensitivity parameter) of these algorithms: some actors matched response proportions to received reinforcer proportions, others overmatched (predominantly chose the highest-valued alternative), and yet others undermatched (chose relatively equally among the alternatives). In each of five experiments, we found that the matchingactor's responses were judged most closely to approximate voluntary choice. We found also that judgments of high volition depended upon stochastic (or probabilistic) generation. Thus, stochastic responses that match reinforcer proportions best represent voluntary human choice.  相似文献   

17.
ABSTRACT

Various religious groups impose strict restrictions on their members marrying someone outside their faith, especially when it comes to Muslims marrying members of other religions. However, travelling in pursuit of higher education and employment has made it possible for people marry to outside their religious affiliation regardless of religious dogma and teachings. Since the 1970s, Turks have been migrating to the UK for various reasons such as job opportunities, and political conflicts happening in their homeland. The increasing number of marriages outside of the Turkish Muslim group has made it necessary to focus on the religious and ethnic identity formation of children in interfaith marriages of couples who are Turkish Muslims and non-Muslims and have at least one child between the ages of five and twenty. This case study examines responses of 32 couples and 15 children collected through questionnaire forms and interviews. The findings demonstrate that gender, religious and ethnic identity, level of religiosity, and the dominant culture have influenced parents’ identity and that of their children. While the parents try to minimise their differences by focusing on moral values, most children develop double consciousness, which can lead to hyphenated identity or syncretism.  相似文献   

18.
Six pigeons were trained on a delayed red-green matching-to-sample task that arranged four delays within sessions. Matching responses intermittently produced either 1.5-s access to food or 4.5-s access to food, and nonmatching responses produced either 1.5-s or 4.5-s blackout. Two phases were conducted: a signaled phase in which the reinforcer magnitudes (small and large) were signaled by houselights (positioned either on the left or right of the chamber), and an unsignaled phase in which there was no correlation between reinforcer magnitude and houselight position. In both phases, the relative frequency with which red and green matching responses produced food was varied across five values. Both matching accuracy and the sensitivity of performance to the distribution of reinforcers for matching responses decreased with increasing delays in both phases. In addition, accuracy and reinforcer sensitivity were significantly lower on signaled small-reinforcer trials compared with accuracy and sensitivity values on signaled large-reinforcer trials and on both types of unsignaled trials. These results are discussed in the context of research on both nonhuman animal and human memory.  相似文献   

19.
Personality, in terms of both strict personality traits (five factors) and their cultural adaptations (e.g., values), has systematically been found to predict religion. This article focuses on three issues that still remain unclear: predictiveness of personality facets versus the five factors; predictiveness of values versus personality; and similarities and differences between religiosity and spirituality in their associations with personality and values. We administered the NEO-PI-R, the Schwartz Value Survey, and religious measures to Spanish students (N = 256). The personality facets provided additional and subtler information than the five factors on individual differences in religion and spirituality. When the overlap between personality and values was controlled for, values were almost unique predictors of these differences. Spirituality shared with religion both a prosocial tendency (with even some intensification) and conscientiousness, but not the emphasis on conservation versus openness to change and to experience.  相似文献   

20.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号