共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
2.
3.
4.
《Psychoanalytic Social Work》2013,20(2-3):61-81
The authors present and elaborate their thesis that psychoanalysis is a hermeneutic science concerned with the causal nature of meaning. They contend that psychoanalysis is concerned primarily with uncovering the meaning of experience as unconsciously determined, that is, caused, by fantasy and as revealed by symptomatic effects. The cause-and-effect interrelationship that exists among fantasy, meaning, and symptom derives from certain abnormal experiences of self relative to selfobject that occur during early development. The authors draw upon their clinical studies of patients diagnosed with panic disorder, OCD, and OCPD and present two clinical vignettes for illustrative purposes. Using a combined method of "analytic deconstruction" and reconstruction, the authors demonstrate how various psychopathological states are reflective of failures to transform archaic narcissistic fantasies that unconsciously determine the meaning of the symptoms suffered by those diagnosed with these specific self-disorders. 相似文献
5.
6.
J.德里达,当今世界上最负盛名的哲学家之一,于2001年9月14日下午访问了上海社会科学院哲学研究所,进行了学术交流.他先作了简短而精练的讲演,题为"解构与本体论",然后,又在讨论中回答了提问.本文系根据德里达的讲演以及他对与本题有关问题的回答的记录整理而成. 相似文献
7.
Continental Philosophy Review - 相似文献
8.
9.
10.
11.
12.
Richard D. Rieke 《Argumentation》1991,5(1):39-55
A variety of theoretical positions are emerging to explain the judicial process from such perspectives as hermeneutics, semiotics, critical theory and argumentation/rhetoric. They ask such questions as these: What is the source of judicial authority? How do judges arrive at their decisions? By what logic are decisions to be tested? In this essay I argue that a focus on decisions and their justifications alone masks the broader process in which judges, along with all the other relevant groups, engage in a continuing and evolving dialogue to structure their normative universe through the complementary processes of dialectic and rhetoric. Contemporary concepts of argumentation can serve to analyze this process critically. 相似文献
13.
14.
15.
Carlos Thiebaut 《Topoi》2006,25(1-2):109-115
Three philosophical attitudes in dialogue are suggested in answering the question posed by the Journal. An inviting, First
Inner Voice undeerstands philosophy as a shareable theoretical task that can be explained and understood even across distant
philosophical paradigms. A Second Inner Voice, sometimes termed in the dialogue as sceptic, distrusts any metaphilosophical
definition of what philosophy is and what it should do, but would, nevertheless, aspire to retain a certain universalistic
understanding of its own work, though it cannot be strongly and conceptualy rendered. A Third Inner Voice, regarded in the
text as somewhat Hegelian, insists in the unavoidability of strong philosophical definitions both in historical and in conceptual
terms. No proposal or conclusion is forwarded regarding what should be done in contemporary philosophy, though an analysis
of harm experiences is taken as an example of philosophical work 相似文献
16.
Jim Mackenzie 《Studia Logica》1990,49(4):567-583
The paper describes four dialogue systems, developed in the tradition of Charles Hamblin. The first system provides an answer for Achilles in Lewis Carroll's parable, the second an analysis of the fallacy of begging the question, the third a non-psychologistic account of conversational implicature, and the fourth an analysis of equivocation and of objections to it. Each avoids combinatorial explosions, and is intended for real-time operation. 相似文献
17.
18.
针对有的学者把德里达解构主义看作当今“新乘”佛教的状况及学界对德里达思想误读的现象,提出以佛家思想为参照系,对两者在世界“有”“无”关系的本体论问题、世界存在方式问题及语言观等方面进行比较,并强调两者的不同之处是占主流的。此外,对德里达逝世之后的解构主义前景作了展望。 相似文献
19.
20.
Group discussion as interactive dialogue or as serial monologue: the influence of group size 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Current models draw a broad distinction between communication as dialogue and communication as monologue. The two kinds of models have different implications for who influences whom in a group discussion. If the discussion is like interactive dialogue, group members should be influenced most by those with whom they interact in the discussion; if it is like serial monologue, they should be influenced most by the dominant speaker. The experiments reported here show that in small, 5-person groups, the communication is like dialogue and members are influenced most by those with whom they interact in the discussion. However, in large, 10-person groups, the communication is like monologue and members are influenced most by the dominant speaker. The difference in mode of communication is explained in terms of how speakers in the two sizes of groups design their utterances for different audiences. 相似文献