首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 46 毫秒
1.
There has been considerable focus on investigating age-related memory changes in cognitively healthy older adults, in the absence of neurodegenerative disorders. Previous studies have reported age-related domain-specific changes in older adults, showing increased difficulty encoding and processing object information but minimal to no impairment in processing spatial information compared with younger adults. However, few of these studies have examined age-related changes in the encoding of concurrently presented object and spatial stimuli, specifically the integration of both spatial and nonspatial (object) information. To more closely resemble real-life memory encoding and the integration of both spatial and nonspatial information, the current study developed a new experimental paradigm with novel environments that allowed for the placement of different objects in different positions within the environment. The results show that older adults have decreased performance in recognizing changes of the object position within the spatial context but no significant differences in recognizing changes in the identity of the object within the spatial context compared with younger adults. These findings suggest there may be potential age-related differences in the mechanisms underlying the representations of complex environments and furthermore, the integration of spatial and nonspatial information may be differentially processed relative to independent and isolated representations of object and spatial information.

Advancing age is associated with changes in a number of cognitive domains (Erickson and Barnes 2003; Salthouse 2004; Craik and Bialystok 2006). Particularly age-related changes in episodic memory function have been frequently reported (Grady and Craik 2000; Craik and Bialystok 2006). In addition to a general decline in long-term retention, older adults show a reduced ability to differentiate between highly similar object representations (Yassa et al. 2011; Stark et al. 2013, 2015; Reagh et al. 2016; Berron et al. 2018) and object features (Yeung et al. 2017) compared with young adults. In contrast, spatial representations appear to be relatively spared (Fidalgo et al. 2016; Stark and Stark 2017) with older adults showing performance similar to young adults recognizing subtle changes in a spatial environment (Berron et al. 2018) and changes in the location of an object when presented on a blank screen (Reagh et al. 2016, 2018).The dissociation and integration of object and spatial information has been a key question in memory research. Older adults show significant impairments in memory binding and maintaining associations despite having intact memory for the individual items (Chalfonte and Johnson 1996; Naveh-Benjamin 2000; Old and Naveh-Benjamin 2008). This is also observed in object-location binding with impairments in recalling the specific location of objects (Kessels et al. 2007; Berger-Mandelbaum and Magen 2019; Muffato et al. 2019) as well as recalling the identity of an object within an environment (Schiavetto et al. 2002; Kessels et al. 2007; Mazurek et al. 2015). The binding of object-location information has been hypothesized to involve the medial temporal lobes, including the hippocampus (Postma et al. 2008), although age-related changes in the prefrontal cortex, posterior neocortex and other regions have also been implicated in object location and object identity tasks (Schiavetto et al. 2002; Meulenbroek et al. 2010). In the medial temporal lobes, the integration of object and spatial information is thought to arise from two parallel information processing streams (Eichenbaum 1999; Eichenbaum et al. 1999; Davachi 2006; Ranganath and Ritchey 2012; Knierim et al. 2013). One pathway, commonly referred to as the “what” pathway involves the perirhinal cortex and the lateral entorhinal cortex, and is thought to predominately process information about objects, items and events, while the “where” pathway involving the parahippocampal cortex and the medial entorhinal cortex is thought to process contextual and spatial information. Information from both pathways is projected to the hippocampus, which is then thought to integrate the spatial and nonspatial information into a cohesive “memory space” through a mechanism that is common to both object or episodic and spatial information (Eichenbaum et al. 1999).However, emerging evidence suggests the processing of object and spatial information may be more integrated than previously thought with the lateral entorhinal cortex processing multimodal information, receiving both object and spatial information (Witter et al. 2017; Doan et al. 2019; Nilssen et al. 2019). In rodent studies, the lateral entorhinal cortex has been reported to be involved in the encoding of features from both the object and the environment (Deshmukh and Knierim 2011; Yoganarasimha et al. 2011; Deshmukh et al. 2012; Knierim et al. 2013). Rodent studies using single cell recordings in the lateral entorhinal cortex show that neurons in this region encode object-related information as well as spatial information about the object (e.g., position in relationship to the environment). These studies also show cells in the lateral entorhinal cortex that track the position of an object in the environment and do not fire when that object is no longer present (Deshmukh and Knierim 2011). A different subset of cells (“object trace cells”) have been reported to fire in previously experienced positions of an object within an environment (Deshmukh and Knierim 2011; Tsao et al. 2013). Lateral entorhinal cortex lesioned rodents show no impairment in performing an object-recognition task, but are impaired at recognizing spatial changes and object changes within a set of objects in an environment including position changes of the objects (Van Cauter et al. 2013) and previously learned object-place and object-context associations (Wilson et al. 2013a; Chao et al. 2016). Together, these findings suggest that, beyond encoding information about objects, the lateral entorhinal cortex encodes contextual information and may be binding nonspatial and spatial information, specifically encoding information about objects and certain spatial properties, including information about the object''s position within environment.Few studies have examined the role of the lateral entorhinal cortex in encoding object identity, object position or changes to the spatial context in humans. Reagh and Yassa (2014) report that subtle perceptual differences between similar objects (e.g., two slightly different apples) elicits activity observed with functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) in both the lateral entorhinal cortex and perirhinal cortex, while changes to an object''s position on a blank screen elicited activity in both the parahippocampal cortex and medial entorhinal cortex. Subsequent studies in older adults show impaired performance recalling the identity of object (Reagh and Yassa 2014; Stark and Stark 2017; Yeung et al. 2017; Berron et al. 2018; Reagh et al. 2018) but similar performance recalling position of the object on a screen compared with young adults (Reagh et al. 2016, 2018). However, these studies examined memory for object identity and object position on a blank screen, devoid of any spatial or contextual information. Given the findings from rodent studies, it appears that object identity and object position information are represented in relationship to the spatial environment in which they occur.To examine the integration of nonspatial and spatial information, novel stimuli were developed to mimic real-life environments where objects could occur within the environment, more closely resembling animal studies in which rodents experience objects within an environment. A series of scenes were designed to have the same perspective, spatial dimensions and outdoor scenery (Fig. 1A). Scenes were classified into five general categories: living room, dining room, kitchen, bedroom, and office rooms to allow for the placement of categorically congruent furniture (Fig. 1B). Critically, within each scene, two to five different positions were defined that an object could logically occupy (Fig. 1C). The scene stimuli were first validated using mnemonic ratings and subsequently used in a novel object-in-context task to assess memory for object identity and object position in context and examine age-related changes in performance on this task in cognitively normal older adults compared with young adults.Open in a separate windowFigure 1.Task stimuli. (A) Scenes were designed to have identical dimensions and a similar perspective. (B) Scenes were designed to have two to five different positions where an object could be reasonably placed. Across all scenes, the same general positions were available for object placement. (C) Example of a scene with an object as seen by the participant. No object was present in the scenes for the stimulus validation study.  相似文献   

2.
Fear-motivated avoidance extinction memory is prone to hippocampal brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF)-dependent reconsolidation upon recall. Here, we show that extinction memory recall activates mammalian target of rapamycin (mTOR) in dorsal CA1, and that post-recall inhibition of this kinase hinders avoidance extinction memory persistence and recovers the learned aversive response. Importantly, coadministration of recombinant BDNF impedes the behavioral effect of hippocampal mTOR inhibition. Our results demonstrate that mTOR signaling is necessary for fear-motivated avoidance extinction memory reconsolidation and suggests that BDNF acts downstream mTOR in a protein synthesis-independent manner to maintain the reactivated extinction memory trace.

Repeated or prolonged nonreinforced recall may induce extinction of consolidated memories, a form of learning involving the formation of a new association that inhibits the expression of the original one (Bouton 2004). On the contrary, brief re-exposure to retrieval cues may destabilize consolidated memories, which must then be reconsolidated to persist (Przybyslawski and Sara 1997; Nader et al. 2000). Psychotherapy based on extinction enhancement or reconsolidation disruption might reduce the intrusive recollection of aversive events and help in the treatment of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), a prevalent mental health condition characterized by the persistent avoidance of places, people, and objects resembling traumatic experiences (Ressler et al. 2004; Schwabe et al. 2014; Dunbar and Taylor 2017; Bryant 2019). Therefore, considerable effort has been lately dedicated to analyze the properties and potential interactions of fear memory extinction and reconsolidation. In this regard, it has been reported that these processes are mutually exclusive (Merlo et al. 2014), and that extinction training during the reconsolidation time window enhances extinction learning and prevents the recovery of fear (Monfils et al. 2009). Moreover, we have previously shown that recall renders fear-motivated avoidance extinction memory susceptible to amnesia, indicating that this memory type is prone to reconsolidation when active and suggesting that targeting extinction memory reconsolidation can be a feasible treatment strategy for PTSD (Rossato et al. 2010; Rosas-Vidal et al. 2015). However, the neurochemical basis of extinction memory reconsolidation has seldom been analyzed.Mammalian target of rapamycin (mTOR) is a 289-kDa phospho-inositide 3-kinase (PI3K)-related serine-threonine protein kinase that functions as a key element of mTOR complex 1 (mTORC1) and mTOR complex 2 (mTORC2) signaling modules to regulate protein synthesis through the phosphorylation of eukaryotic initiation factor 4E (eIF4E)-binding protein 1 (4E-BP1) and p70 ribosomal S6 kinase (p70S6K) (Hay and Sonenberg 2004). A well-known mediator of cell growth and proliferation (Hall 2008; Ryskalin et al. 2017), mTOR involvement in synaptic plasticity was first suggested by studies showing that rapamycin (RAPA), a macrolide that selectively inhibits mTORC1 signaling by interacting with the chaperone FKBP12 and binding to mTOR FKBP12–RAPA-binding domain, impairs long-term facilitation in Aplysia as well as long-term potentiation (LTP) in the rat hippocampus (Casadio et al. 1999; Tang et al. 2002). Interestingly, avoidance memory consolidation and recall need mTOR signaling in the dorsal hippocampus (Bekinschtein et al. 2007; Pereyra et al. 2018), as it also happens with the reconsolidation and extinction of several other memory types (Myskiw et al. 2008; Gafford et al. 2011; Zubedat and Akirav 2017; Jarome et al. 2018; Lee et al. 2018; Yang et al. 2019). Here, we examined whether reconsolidation of fear-motivated avoidance extinction memory requires mTOR activity in the CA1 region of the dorsal hippocampus. To do that, we used 3-mo-old, 300- to 350-g, male Wistar rats (n = 320), housed in groups of five with free access to water and food in a holding room at 22°C–23°C on a normal light cycle (12 h light:12 h dark; lights on at 6.00 a.m.). Animals were implanted with 22-gauge guides aimed at the CA1 region of the dorsal hippocampus (Supplemental Fig. S1, stereotaxic coordinates in millimeters: anteroposterior, −4.2; laterolateral, ±3.0; dorsoventral, −3.0), as previously described (Radiske et al. 2015), and allowed to recover from surgery for 10 d before being handled by the experimenter once per day for 2 d. One day later, the animals were trained in a one-trial step-down inhibitory avoidance (SDIA) task, an aversive learning paradigm in which stepping down from a platform is paired with a mild footshock. Briefly, the SDIA training box (50 × 25 × 25 cm) was made of Plexiglas and fitted with a grid floor through which scrambled electric shocks could be delivered to the rat''s feet. Over the left end of the grid floor there was a 5-cm-high, 8-cm-wide, 25-cm-long wooden platform. For training, the animals were individually placed on the platform facing the left rear corner of the training box and, when they stepped down and placed their four paws on the grid, received a 2-sec, 0.4-mA scrambled footshock, whereupon they were immediately withdrawn from the training box. This training protocol induces a long-lasting, hippocampus-dependent, fear-motivated avoidance memory expressed as an increase in step-down latency at test (Bernabeu et al. 1995; Paratcha et al. 2000; Katche et al. 2013). However, repeated testing in the absence of the footshock causes clear-cut extinction (Cammarota et al. 2005; Rossato et al. 2006; Bonini et al. 2011). Therefore, to extinguish the learned avoidance response, we submitted SDIA trained rats to one daily unreinforced test session for five consecutive days. To that end, we put the animals back on the training box platform until they stepped down to the grid. No footshock was given, and the animals were allowed to freely explore the training apparatus for 30 sec after stepping down. During this time, the animals stepped up onto the platform and down again several times. This procedure induces an SDIA extinction memory immune to spontaneous recovery, reinstatement and renewal that lasts for at least 14 d and requires NMDA receptor activation as well as protein synthesis and gene expression in dorsal CA1 to consolidate (Cammarota et al. 2003; Rossato et al. 2010; Radiske et al. 2015). One day after the last extinction session, extinction memory was reactivated by placing the animals on the training box platform until they stepped down from it. Five minutes or 6 h later, the animals received bilateral intradorsal CA1 infusions (1 µL/side) of vehicle (VEH; 5% DMSO in saline), RAPA (0.02 µg/side) or the selective ATP-competitive inhibitor of mTOR, TORIN2 (TORIN; 0.20 µg/side). RAPA and TORIN were dissolved in DMSO and diluted to working concentration in sterile saline (<5% DMSO). The doses used were determined based on pilot experiments and previous studies showing the behavioral and biochemical effects of each compound (Bekinschtein et al. 2007; Revest et al. 2014; Renard et al. 2016; Lee et al. 2018). Retention was evaluated at different times after extinction memory reactivation by placing the animals on the training box platform and measuring their latency to step down. Because of the 300-sec ceiling imposed on test latency, step-down data were expressed as median ± IQR and analyzed using the Kruskal–Wallis test followed by Dunn''s post hoc comparisons. We found that animals that received VEH recalled SDIA extinction memory normally regardless of the time elapsed between reactivation and test sessions. Conversely, RAPA and TORIN given 5 min, but not 6 h, after SDIA extinction memory reactivation impaired retention of extinction and induced reappearance of the SDIA response 1 d and 7 d later (Fig. 1A, 1 d after RA: H = 24.42, P < 0.001; P < 0.001 for VEH vs. RAPA, P < 0.001 for VEH vs. TORIN; 7 d after RA: H = 26.85, P < 0.001; P < 0.001 for VEH vs. RAPA, P < 0.001 for VEH vs. TORIN in Dunn''s multiple comparisons after Kruskal–Wallis test; Fig. 1B, 1 d after RA: H = 4.510, P = 0.1049; 7 d after RA: H = 4.606, P = 0.0999 in Kruskal–Wallis test). Neither RAPA nor TORIN affected SDIA extinction memory when administered 24 h after the last extinction session in the absence of extinction memory reactivation (Fig. 1C, 1 d after infusion: H = 2.141, P = 0.3428; 7 d after infusion: H = 4.086, P = 0.1296 in Kruskal–Wallis test) or when given 5 min post-reactivation but retention was evaluated 3 h thereafter (Fig. 1D, H = 1.654, P = 0.4375 in Kruskal–Wallis test). Moreover, RAPA and TORIN had no effect on extinction memory retention if injected in dorsal CA1 5 min after an extinction pseudoreactivation session carried out in an avoidance training box rendered nonaversive for SDIA-trained animals (Fig. 1E, After RA: H = 13.86, P = 0.001; P < 0.01 for VEH vs. RAPA, P < 0.01 for VEH vs. TORIN; After PseudoRA: H = 0.7503, P = 0.6872 in Dunn''s multiple comparisons after Kruskal–Wallis test; Supplemental Fig. S2). mTOR activity is regulated by phosphorylation at different sites (Watanabe et al. 2011). Phosphorylation at Ser2448 is mediated by p70S6K, occurs mainly to mTOR associated with mTORC1 (Chiang and Abraham 2005; Holz and Blenis 2005; Akcakanat et al. 2007), enables mTOR binding to regulatory-associated protein of mTOR (RAPTOR), and correlates with mTORC1 activation (Rosner et al. 2010). On the contrary, Ser2481 is an autophosphorylation site insensitive to acute rapamycin treatment that is phosphorylated only when mTOR makes part of mTORC2 complexes (Peterson et al. 2000; Copp et al. 2009). To analyze mTOR phosphorylation levels, we performed immunoblotting on total homogenates from the CA1 region of the dorsal hippocampus. Samples were not pooled. Equal amounts of proteins (15 µg) were fractionated by SDS-PAGE and transferred to PVDF membranes. Blots were blocked for 1 h, incubated overnight at 4°C with anti-pSer2448 mTOR (1:10,000; RRID:AB_330970), anti-pSer2481 mTOR (1:10,000; RRID:AB_2262884), or anti-mTOR (1:10,000; RRID:AB_330978), and then incubated for 2 h at room temperature with HRP-coupled anti-IgG secondary antibody. Immunoreactivity was detected using the Amersham ECL Prime Western Blotting Detection Reagent and the Amersham Imager 600 system. Densitometric analyses were performed using the ImageQuant TL 8.1 analysis software (GE Healthcare). We found that pSer2448 mTOR levels peaked 5 min after SDIA extinction memory reactivation and returned to control values within 30 min (Fig. 2, F(5,20) = 2.805, P = 0.0446; P < 0.05 for 5 min vs. No RA in Dunnett''s multiple comparison test after repeated measures ANOVA). No changes in pSer2481 mTOR or total mTOR levels were found up to 6 h post-reactivation (Fig. 2, pSer2481 mTOR: F(5,20) = 1.241, P = 0.3274; mTOR: F(5,20) = 1.208, P = 0.3411 in repeated measures ANOVA; Supplemental Fig. S3). mTORC1 activation stimulates brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) production in hippocampal neurons (Jeon et al. 2015), which in turn may induce mTOR-dependent activation of dendritic mRNA translation (Takei et al. 2004). Previously, we reported that hippocampal BDNF maintains fear-motivated avoidance extinction memory after recall (Radiske et al. 2015). In agreement with this finding, coinfusion of recombinant BDNF (0.25 µg/side) after SDIA extinction memory reactivation impeded the recovery of the avoidance response provoked by RAPA (Fig. 3, 1 d after RA: H = 27.52, P < 0.001; P < 0.001 for VEH vs. RAPA, P < 0.001 for BDNF vs. RAPA, P < 0.05 for RAPA vs. RAPA + BDNF; 7 d after RA: H = 26.76, P < 0.001; P < 0.001 for VEH vs. RAPA, P < 0.001 for BDNF vs. RAPA, P < 0.01 for RAPA vs. RAPA + BDNF in Dunn''s multiple comparisons after Kruskal–Wallis test).Open in a separate windowFigure 1.mTOR is required for fear-motivated avoidance extinction memory reconsolidation. (A) Animals were trained in SDIA (TR; 0.4 mA/2 sec) and beginning 24 h later submitted to one daily extinction session for five consecutive days (EXT). Twenty-four hours after the last session, extinction memory was reactivated (RA) and, 5 min thereafter, the animals received bilateral intradorsal CA1 infusions of vehicle (VEH; 5% DMSO in saline), rapamycin (RAPA; 0.02 µg/side) or TORIN (0.20 µg/side). Retention was assessed 1 and 7 d later (Test). (B) Animals were treated as in A except that they received intra-CA1 infusions of VEH, RAPA, or TORIN 6 h after RA. (C) Animals were treated as in A, except that they received VEH, RAPA, or TORIN in dorsal CA1 24 h after the last extinction session in the absence of RA (No RA). (D) Animals were treated as described in A, except that VEH, RAPA, or TORIN were given 5 min after RA and retention was assessed 3 h later. (E) Animals were treated as in A, except that a subgroup of animals received VEH, RAPA, or TORIN 5 min after an extinction pseudoreactivation session in an avoidance training box rendered nonaversive for SDIA-trained animals. The nonaversive box was similar in dimensions to the SDIA training box, but it was made of dark gray wood and had a Plexiglas platform. (PRA) Pseudoreactivation session. Data are expressed as median ± IQR. (**) P < 0.01, (***) P < 0.001 versus VEH in Dunn''s multiple comparisons after Kruskal–Wallis test.Open in a separate windowFigure 2.Reactivation of fear-motivated avoidance extinction memory increases mTOR phosphorylation at Ser2448, but not at Ser2481, in the CA1 region of the dorsal hippocampus. Animals were trained in SDIA (0.4 mA/2 s) and beginning 24 h later submitted to one daily extinction session for 5 consecutive days. Twenty-four hours after the last session, extinction memory was reactivated (RA) and the animals killed by decapitation at different post-reactivation times (5–360 min). The CA1 region of the dorsal hippocampus was dissected out, homogenized, and used to determine of pS2448 mTOR, pS2481 mTOR, or mTOR levels by immunoblotting. (N) Naïve animals, (No RA) animals trained in SDIA that were submitted to five daily extinction sessions and killed 24 h after the last extinction session. Data are expressed as mean ± SEM. (*) P < 0.05 versus No RA in Dunnett''s multiple comparison test after repeated measures ANOVA.Open in a separate windowFigure 3.Coinfusion of recombinant BDNF reverses the effect of RAPA on fear-motivated avoidance extinction memory reconsolidation. Animals were trained in SDIA (TR; 0.4 mA/2 sec) and beginning 24 h later were submitted to one daily extinction session for five consecutive days (EXT). Twenty-four hours after the last session, extinction memory was reactivated (RA) and 5 min later the animals received bilateral intradorsal CA1 infusions of vehicle (VEH; 5% DMSO in saline), rapamycin (RAPA; 0.02 µg/side), BDNF (0.25 µg/µL), or RAPA plus BDNF (RAPA + BDNF). Retention was assessed 1 and 7 d later (Test). Data expressed as median ± IQR. (***) P < 0.001 versus VEH in Dunn''s multiple comparisons after Kruskal–Wallis test.Our results show that dorsal CA1 mTOR inhibition during a short post-recall time window persistently impairs retention of SDIA extinction memory and causes avoidance reappearance. This effect took time to develop, was time-dependent, concomitant with SDIA extinction memory reactivation, and occurred after the administration of mTOR inhibitors with different mechanisms of action, suggesting that it was not spontaneous or caused by nonspecific pharmacological interactions but due to bona fide impairment of an active mTOR-dependent reconsolidation process. This conclusion is further supported by findings showing that SDIA extinction memory reactivation rapidly and transiently increased mTOR phosphorylation at Ser2448, a post-translational modification customarily used as a proxy for mTOR activation (Reynolds et al. 2002; Guertin and Sabatini 2007; Rivas et al. 2009; Guo et al. 2017; Dong et al. 2018; Rosa et al. 2019). Most findings indicate that BDNF modulates protein synthesis through mTOR (Takei et al. 2001, 2004). In fact, BDNF controls hippocampal synaptic mRNA translation by regulating mTORC activation state (Briz et al. 2013; Leal et al. 2014), which seems to be necessary for SDIA memory consolidation (Slipczuk et al. 2009). However, in agreement with previous findings that BDNF is sufficient to restabilize a reactivated extinction memory trace, even when hippocampal protein synthesis and gene expression are inhibited (Radiske et al. 2015), our results show that mTOR acts upstream BDNF during the reconsolidation of extinction, and suggest not only that BDNF is a key protein synthesis product for this process but also that its actions are not mediated by mTOR-dependent mRNA translation. Indeed, mTOR signaling controls BDNF activity-dependent dendritic translation (Baj et al. 2016), and several protein synthesis-dependent plastic mechanisms, including late-LTP and memory consolidation, are rescued by BDNF when protein synthesis is impaired (Pang and Lu 2004; Moguel-González et al. 2008; Martínez-Moreno et al. 2011; Ozawa et al. 2014). Exogenous BDNF becomes quickly available for activity-dependent secretion, rapidly replacing the endogenous biosynthetic pathway after its administration (Santi et al. 2006). Thus, the rapid modulation of hippocampal high-frequency transmission produced by this neurotrophin is unaffected by protein synthesis inhibitors (Gottschalk et al. 1999; Tartaglia et al. 2001) and BDNF administration may induce the lasting structural reorganization and potentiation of hippocampal synapses in an mRNA synthesis and protein translation-independent manner (Martínez-Moreno et al. 2020), perhaps through a mechanism involving PKMζ activity regulation (Mei et al. 2011). In fact, hippocampal PKMζ acts downstream BDNF to control AMPAR synaptic insertion through a protein synthesis-independent mechanism during declarative memory reconsolidation (Rossato et al. 2019).In conclusion, our results confirm that extinction does not erase the SDIA response but generates an inhibitory memory that coexists with it and controls its expression. The data also corroborate that avoidance extinction memory enters a labile state when reactivated by recall and needs to be reconsolidated through a mechanism involving hippocampal mTOR/BDNF signaling activation to maintain its dominance over the aversive trace. Finally, though not less important, our findings emphasize the necessity of understanding the dynamics of memory competition in order to develop better therapeutic strategies for PTSD treatment.  相似文献   

3.
Spatial memory comprises different representational systems that are sensitive to different environmental cues, like proximal landmarks or local boundaries. Here we examined how sleep affects the formation of a spatial representation integrating landmark-referenced and boundary-referenced representations. To this end, participants (n = 42) were familiarized with an environment featuring both a proximal landmark and a local boundary. After nocturnal periods of sleep or wakefulness and another night of sleep, integration of the two representational systems was tested by testing the participant''s flexibility to switch from landmark-based to boundary-based navigation in the environment, and vice versa. Results indicate a distinctly increased flexibility in relying on either landmarks or boundaries for navigation, when familiarization to the environment was followed by sleep rather than by wakefulness. A second control study (n = 45) did not reveal effects of sleep (vs. wakefulness) on navigation in environments featuring only landmarks or only boundaries. Thus, rather than strengthening isolated representational systems per se, sleep presumably through forming an integrative representation, enhances flexible coordination of representational subsystems.

Wilson and McNaughton (1994) reported that “… information acquired during active behavior is … reexpressed in hippocampal circuits during sleep….” This observation of experience-dependent neural replay activity in the brain during slow-wave sleep (for review, see O''Neill et al. 2010) forms a keystone in our current understanding of how sleep affects memory consolidation in an active system consolidation process that involves the redistribution of hippocampal memory to extrahippocampal regions (McClelland et al. 1995; Diekelmann and Born 2010; Klinzing et al. 2019). According to theory, the emerging extrahippocampal memory representations are essentially schematic, devoid of specific context-information, and lack minute detail (Lewis and Durrant 2011; Payne 2011; Sekeres et al. 2018). Simultaneously, hippocampal replay strengthens hippocampal memory traces in the short-term following Hebbian learning, leading to improved context memory immediately after sleep compared with wakefulness (van der Helm et al. 2011; Weber et al. 2014). In the present study, we sought to test sleep''s role in establishing higher-level memory representations drawing on the example of spatial memory processing.Inspired by the strong role of the hippocampal formation in human spatial memory (Burgess 2008; Hartley et al. 2014) a number of studies examined effects of sleep specifically on spatial memory consolidation (Peigneux et al. 2004; Orban et al. 2006; Ferrara et al. 2008; Rauchs et al. 2008; Wamsley et al. 2010; Nguyen et al. 2013; Noack et al. 2017). In these studies, participants explored a virtual environment during a learning phase before retention periods of sleep and wakefulness and, later on, engaged in specific retrieval tasks that required to reach a predefined goal location in the environment as fast as possible. Results were mixed with, some studies reporting positive effects of sleep on spatial navigation performance (e.g., Peigneux et al. 2004; Wamsley et al. 2010; Nguyen et al. 2013; Noack et al. 2017), whereas in others such sleep effect depended on the length of the retention interval (e.g., Ferrara et al. 2008), or was completely absent (Orban et al. 2006; Rauchs et al. 2008). Interestingly, in the latter studies—despite absent behavioral effects—using a 72-h retention interval between learning and retrieval testing, functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) suggested that sleep favors a shift from activation of hippocampal areas toward preferential activation of striatal areas at retrieval of the relevant spatial representations.Indeed, spatial navigation can rely on two distinct representational systems that involve as key structures hippocampal and striatal circuitry, respectively, and are also linked to different spatial frames of reference (Burgess 2008; Hartley et al. 2014). Doeller et al. (2008) showed in humans that striatal activation is linked to the processing of single proximal landmarks whereas hippocampal activation is related to the processing of spatial boundaries, and that acquisition of representations in both systems may follow different learning rules (Doeller and Burgess 2008). The subject''s reliance on one or the other representation system depends on the specific navigational problem (Maguire et al. 1998; Hartley et al. 2003) as well as familiarity with the environment (Hartley et al. 2003; Iaria et al. 2003; Packard and McGaugh 1996), but both systems can also be activated in parallel and interact. For example, patients with hippocampal atrophy showed impaired memory performance not only for boundary-based but also for landmark-based navigation (Guderian et al. 2015) suggesting the presence of synergistic effects between the representational systems. The activation of the representational systems is presumably coordinated by the medial prefrontal cortex (Ragozzino et al. 1999; Doeller et al. 2008; Rich and Shapiro 2009), that is, a region that is not only involved in the abstraction of schema-like spatial representations (Tse et al. 2011; van Buuren et al. 2014) but, also shows neuronal reactivation during sleep (Euston et al. 2007; Peyrache et al. 2009).In fact, there is first evidence suggesting that sleep supports the formation of abstract representations of space in particular. We found, for example, that sleep benefitted the extraction of semantic structure (regions defined by semantic category of landmarks) in a virtual navigation task (Noack et al. 2017). To date, there is no study, however, to specifically test the interaction between landmark- and boundary-referenced representations of space and their integration during sleep. Here we sought to fill this gap. Drawing on the active systems consolidation concept of sleep (Dudai et al. 2015; Klinzing et al. 2019) and on the existing literature, we followed the hypothesis that, rather than benefiting a specific spatial representation, sleep via neuronal replay primarily supports the formation of an integrative schema-like spatial representation and, thereby, improves flexibility in the use of hippocampus-based and striatum-based representations.To this end, we conducted two experiments, a Main experiment and a Control experiment, using a virtual spatial environment with one proximal landmark and a local boundary (Fig. 1) to preferentially engage striatum and hippocampus-based representational systems, respectively (Doeller et al. 2008). The Main experiment was designed to test the effect of sleep on the integration of landmark-referenced and boundary-referenced representations of space. To this end, participants were first familiarized with an environment featuring both a landmark and a boundary, thereby encoding both hippocampal as well as striatal representations of the environment. In order to test whether sleep enhances the integration of these representations, participants either slept or remained awake on the night after the Familiarization phase. They then learned new objects in impoverished environments featuring the same spatial cues (landmark and boundary) at the same locations but only one at a time. At a final Test session, the integration of the combined environmental layout including landmark and boundary (as presented during Familiarization before sleep) was investigated by the participant''s flexibility to switch from landmark-based to boundary-based navigation in the environment, and vice versa, from boundary-based to landmark-based navigation (Fig. 1). In the Control experiment, we investigated the direct effect of postlearning sleep or wakefulness on the consolidation of spatial memory representation that were either merely boundary-referenced or landmark-referenced, thereby controlling for general effects of sleep on spatial memory performance.Open in a separate windowFigure 1.Task and general procedures. (A) Example views on the three different environments. (Panel i) landmark and boundary present, as used in the Familiarization phase of the Main experiment. Alpine environment (panel ii), and Desert environment (panel iii) as used in the Control experiment. (B) Task procedure: The task featured three different trial types in both experiments. (Panel i) Acquisition trials were presented at the start of Familiarization and Learning phases in both experiments. (Panel ii) Feedback and Test trials started with the presentation of an object on a gray screen. Participants were then placed in the experimental environment containing boundary (thick encirclement), landmarks (traffic cone) or both, and dropped the object at the location where they found it during acquisition. In Feedback trials feedback was given by presenting the object at its correct location. Participants navigated to it to collect it. (C) Design of Main experiment: Environment featured both landmark and boundary cues during Familiarization. The Test session comprised Learning phase and Retrieval phase. Only one spatial cue (landmark or boundary) was present during each trial of the Learning and Retrieval phase (three objects with landmark, three objects with boundary). Object reference switched from Learning to Retrieval phase: Objects presented together with the landmark during learning were presented with boundary during retrieval and vice versa. Note that a specific spatial cue was always at the same relative position when presented during Familiarization, Learning, and Test. (D) Design of Control experiment: Participants were randomly assigned to the Boundary or the Landmark group, whereas all participants performed in Wake and Sleep condition. Each of the two visits (sleep and wake) consisted of two sessions (learning: six Acquisition trials + four blocks and six feedback trials; retrieval: three blocks and six Test trials).To preview our results: Whereas there was no effect of sleep on landmark- and boundary-referenced spatial memory per se in the Control experiment, sleep indeed facilitated the flexible use of different spatial retrieval cues possibly based on a superior integrated spatial memory representation.  相似文献   

4.
Remembering sequences of events defines episodic memory, but retrieval can be driven by both ordinality and temporal contexts. Whether these modes of retrieval operate at the same time or not remains unclear. Theoretically, medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) confers ordinality, while the hippocampus (HC) associates events in gradually changing temporal contexts. Here, we looked for evidence of each with BOLD fMRI in a sequence task that taxes both retrieval modes. To test ordinal modes, items were transferred between sequences but retained their position (e.g., AB3). Ordinal modes activated mPFC, but not HC. To test temporal contexts, we examined items that skipped ahead across lag distances (e.g., ABD). HC, but not mPFC, tracked temporal contexts. There was a mPFC and HC by retrieval mode interaction. These current results suggest that the mPFC and HC are concurrently engaged in different retrieval modes in support of remembering when an event occurred.

Memory for sequences of events is a fundamental component of episodic memory (Tulving 1984, 2002; Allen and Fortin 2013; Howard and Eichenbaum 2013; Eichenbaum 2017). While different experiences share overlapping elements, the sequence of events is unique. Remembering the order of events allows us to disambiguate episodes with similar content and make detailed predictions supporting decision-making.At least two complementary memory processes contribute to the retrieval of events in the correct sequence: ordinal (Orlov et al. 2002) and temporal context (Howard and Kahana 2002) retrieval modes. Whether these disparate retrieval modes operate coincidently or not remains an open question with consequences for understanding basic mechanisms of how we remember the events that unfold throughout our day. According to an ordinal retrieval mode, items are remembered by their position within an event sequence (DuBrow and Davachi 2013; Allen et al. 2014; Long and Kahana 2019), providing sequential memory through well-established semantic or abstracted relationships (first, second, third, etc.). While for a temporal context retrieval mode, events are remembered through a gradually changing temporal context within which specific items have been associated. According to temporal contexts, when an element of a sequence is presented or retrieved (e.g., “C” in ABCDEF), items that are more proximal in the sequence (e.g., the “D” in the sequence) have a higher retrieval rate compared with items that are further away (e.g., the “F” in the sequence). These temporal contexts result from item associations that are dependent on time varying neural activity (e.g., Eichenbaum 2014), and contribute to sequence memory through the reactivation of neighboring items during retrieval (DuBrow and Davachi 2013; Long and Kahana 2019).The medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) and hippocampus (HC) are thought to contribute to sequence memory through ordinal representations and temporal contexts, respectively (Agster et al. 2002; Fortin et al. 2002; Kesner et al. 2002; DeVito and Eichenbaum 2011; Allen et al. 2016; Jenkins and Ranganath 2016). In rodents, mPFC disruptions impair sequence memory (DeVito and Eichenbaum 2011; Jayachandran et al. 2019), mPFC “time cells” are evident (Tiganj et al. 2017), and positions within a sequence can be the main determinant of differential activity in mPFC neurons during spatial sequences (Euston and McNaughton 2006). In humans, mPFC activation is sensitive to temporal order memory (Preston and Eichenbaum 2013), and codes for information about temporal positions within image sequences regardless of the image itself (Hsieh and Ranganath 2015). HC activations are also generally associated with temporal order memory (Kumaran and Maguire 2006; Ekstrom and Bookheimer 2007; Lehn et al. 2009; Ross et al. 2009; Jenkins and Ranganath 2010; Tubridy and Davachi 2011; Kalm et al. 2013; Hsieh et al. 2014; Goyal et al. 2018). Prior evidence further shows that the medial temporal lobe, specifically the HC formation, plays a critical role in the use of a TCM retrieval mode in the brain (Manns et al. 2007; Hsieh et al. 2014; Bladon et al. 2019). The HC binds events within temporal contexts (Eichenbaum et al. 2007; DuBrow and Davachi 2013; Bladon et al. 2019) through a gradually changing neural context (Manns et al. 2007; Mankin et al. 2012). Similarly, medial temporal lobe neuronal and BOLD activations in humans have demonstrated evidence for gradually evolving temporal contexts (Howard et al. 2012; Kalm et al. 2013; Kragel et al. 2015).Here we tested the contributions of the mPFC and HC during a visual sequence memory task that provides behavioral evidence of both ordinal and temporal context retrieval modes (see Fig. 1A; task modified from Allen et al. 2014). Briefly, participants first memorized six visual sequences (six images each) in a single passive viewing phase, and then were instructed to make judgments as to whether individual items were subsequently presented in sequence (InSeq) or out of sequence (OutSeq) over 240 self-paced presentations of each of the six items from each sequence. In the task, the two retrieval modes are parsed using probe trials that place conflicting demands on ordinal (Orlov et al. 2000; Allen et al. 2014, 2015) and temporal context modes (Jayachandran et al. 2019). We first evaluated ordinal retrieval modes using items that were transferred from one sequence to another while retaining their ordinal position (Ordinal Transfers) (Fig. 1B). Evidence for an ordinal-based retrieval mode occurs when these probes are identified as in sequence, because they occur in the same ordinal position as their original sequence. mPFC activations (but not HC) was strongest for these ordinal retrievals. Second, we evaluated a temporal context retrieval mode using items that skipped ahead (Skips) (Fig. 1B) with shorter lag distances (ABCFEF) compared with larger lag distances (AFCDEF). Skips should be most difficult to detect on the shortest lag distances because proximal items in a sequence are more likely to be retrieved (Howard and Kahana 2002; Kragel et al. 2015) and thus judged as InSeq. HC activations (but not mPFC) tracked with lag distance, providing evidence the HC is more reflective of a temporal context-based retrieval mode. Importantly, a significant interaction was observed such that mPFC and HC differentially activated for ordinal and temporal context retrievals. Altogether, our data show that sequence memory involves both retrieval modes. In line with these results, we suggest that understanding episodic memory requires more insight into the neurobiology of ordinal processing, in addition to the more often studied temporal contexts, in the mPFC and HC system.Open in a separate windowFigure 1.Sequence memory task and overall performance levels. Participants were tested on a sequence memory task that differentially burdens different retrieval modes using different out of sequence probe trial types. (A) An example sequence set that included six sequences. Two sequences were low memory demand sequences and four were high memory demand sequences. (B) There were three out of sequence probe trial types: items that were repeated in the sequence (Repeats), items that were presented too early in the sequence (Skips), and items that transferred from one sequence to another, while remaining in their ordinal position (Ordinal Transfers). Repeats and Skips occurred throughout the whole task, whereas Ordinal Transfers occurred during the second half only. (C) Accuracy throughout the task (error bars = ±1SD). Participants performed best on Repeats, then Skips, and poorest on Ordinal Transfers. (D,E) Distributions of response times for all InSeq trials (D, gray bars) and for all OutSeq trials (E, gray bars) for all participants with a fitted two-term Gaussian curve (black line). (F) A bimodal Gaussian curve fit better than a unimodal curve for InSeq and OutSeq trials. A trimodal curve did not improve the fit and increased the root mean squared error (not shown), suggesting distinct decisions decision-making between two decisions. It was rare to observe responses outside of the two distributions.  相似文献   

5.
Conditioned stimuli (CS) have multiple psychological functions that can potentially contribute to their effect on memory formation. It is generally believed that CS-induced memory modulation is primarily due to conditioned emotional responses, however, well-learned CSs not only generate the appropriate behavioral and physiological reactions required to best respond to an upcoming unconditioned stimulus (US), but they also serve as signals that the US is about to occur. Therefore, it is possible that CSs can impact memory consolidation even when their ability to elicit conditioned emotional arousal is significantly reduced. To test this, male Sprague–Dawley rats trained on a signaled active avoidance task were divided into “Avoider” and “Non-Avoider” subgroups on the basis of percentage avoidance after 6 d of training. Subgroup differences in responding to the CS complex were maintained during a test carried out in the absence of the US. Moreover, the subgroups displayed significant differences in stress-induced analgesia (hot-plate test) immediately after this test, suggesting significant subgroup differences in conditioned emotionality. Importantly, using the spontaneous object recognition task, it was found that immediate post-sample exposure to the avoidance CS complex had a similar enhancing effect on object memory in the two subgroups. Therefore, to our knowledge, this is the first study to demonstrate that a significant conditioned emotional response is not necessary for the action of a predictive CS on modulation of memory consolidation.

Biologically significant stimuli (unconditioned stimuli [US]) support learning and promote changes in behavior by enhancing the consolidation of memory (White and Milner 1992). Thus, stimuli such as food (Huston et al. 1974, 1977), pain (Galvez et al. 1996; Quirarte et al. 1998), and various drugs of abuse (Krivanek and McGaugh 1969; White 1996; Leri et al. 2013; Rkieh et al. 2014; Wolter et al. 2019, 2020) increase memory storage and facilitate performance on a variety of learning tasks when delivered during a window of memory consolidation that occurs following a learning experience (McGaugh and Roozendaal 2009; Roozendaal and Mcgaugh 2012; McGaugh 2015).Interestingly, exposure to stimuli paired with both incentive (Holahan and White 2013; Wolter et al. 2019, 2020; Baidoo et al. 2020) and aversive (Holahan and White 2002, 2004; Leong et al. 2015; Goode et al. 2016) USs also enhances memory consolidation, presumably because of the conditioned emotional responses that they generate. For example, CSs that precede exposure to footshock elicit freezing (Díaz-Mataix et al. 2017), avoidance (Dombrowski et al. 2013), analgesia (McNally et al. 1999), as well as sympathetic stimulation such as increases in heart rate (Zhang et al. 2019), blood pressure (Hsu et al. 2012), and release of stress hormones (Feenstra et al. 1999), all reactions that are elicited by footshock itself (Lim et al. 1982; McCarty and Baucom 1982; Conti et al. 1990; Galvez et al. 1996; O''Doherty 2004; Lázaro-Muñoz et al. 2010). Holahan and White (2002, 2004) reported that the memory enhancing action of a shock-paired CS could be blocked by lesions of the central amygdala nucleus (CeA), a region involved in generating the behavioral and neurohormonal responses to emotionally arousing stimuli (LeDoux 2003). As well, similarly to a range of aversive USs (anxiogenic drugs, predator odor, tail shock, restraint stress; Kim et al. 2001; Elliott and Packard 2008; Leong and Packard 2014), the effect of a CS paired with footshock on consolidation was found dependent on noradrenergic activation of the amygdala (Goode et al. 2016).However, well-learned CSs not only generate the appropriate behavioral and physiological reactions required to best respond to an upcoming US, but they also serve as signals that the US is about to occur. Temporal relationships between CSs and USs are learned rapidly during conditioning (Ohyama and Mauk 2001; Balsam et al. 2002), and these expectations modulate the expression of learned responses (Holland 2000; Balsam et al. 2010). Moreover, the ability of CSs to predict USs is heavily dependent on mesolimbic dopamine (DA) activity (Schultz et al. 1997; Flagel et al. 2011), and there is substantial evidence that mid-brain DA plays an important role in memory consolidation (White 1989; Managò et al. 2009; Redondo and Morris 2011; Yamasaki and Takeuchi 2017).This analysis suggests that CSs can impact memory consolidation because of their predictive function, even when their ability to elicit preparatory conditioned emotionality is significantly reduced. To test this idea, the current study used a signaled active avoidance task whereby rats learn to avoid an aversive US (footshock) by crossing from one compartment of a shuttle box to another during the presentation of a warning signal. Miller (1948) posited that animals perform the shuttle response during the signal because it prevents the occurrence of the US, and this reduces the experience of conditioned fear caused by the signal. However, it has been found that avoidance persists even when the warning signal no longer elicits a measurable fear state (Kamin et al. 1963; Linden 1969; Coover and Ursin 1973; Starr and Mineka 1977; Mineka and Gino 1980), suggesting that CSs can promote robust avoidance even though conditioned emotional responses are greatly reduced. The current study also used active avoidance because it consistently reveals robust individual differences in learning (Choi et al. 2010; Lázaro-Muñoz et al. 2010; Martinez et al. 2013; Antunes et al. 2020), such that animals can be distinguished into subgroups of Avoiders and Non-avoiders by simple median split (Storace et al. 2019) on percentage avoided USs. Although the source of the individual differences is unknown, it has been postulated the subgroups learn different behavioral responses to the CS (Lázaro-Muñoz et al. 2010; Martinez et al. 2013; Antunes et al. 2020): Avoiders display a loss of fear responses to the CS as the avoidance response is acquired (LeDoux et al. 2017; Cain 2018), while those who fail to acquire the avoidance response continue to display conditioned fear characterized by freezing (Martinez et al. 2013).By capitalizing on these individual differences, the current study explored whether both the predictive and preparatory functions of aversive CSs play a role in modulating consolidation of object memory using the spontaneous object recognition (OR) task. OR relies on the natural tendency of rats to explore novel objects (Winters et al. 2008) and this task was selected because it has been found sensitive to enhancement by exposure to contextual CSs paired with both incentive and aversive stimuli (Wolter et al. 2019, 2020; Baidoo et al. 2020). Given the evidence reviewed above, it was predicted that exposure to the avoidance CS complex (the training chamber, the retractable gate, the warning tone, and the cue light) would impact consolidation of object memory equally in Avoider and Non-Avoider subgroups. Avoider and Non-Avoider subgroups were tested for reactivity to thermal pain throughout avoidance training and testing using the hot-plate to provide an indirect measure of emotional reactivity to the footshock and/or to the aversive CS complex (Fig. 1). This approach was selected because fear/stress-inducing stimuli such as footshock (Maier and Watkins 1991; Rosellini et al. 1994), predator odor (Williams et al. 2005), and their CSs (Hotsenpiller and Williams 1997; McNally and Akil 2001; Ford et al. 2011), elicit stress-induced analgesia; a well-known defensive response in various species (Bolles and Fanselow 1980; Fendt and Fanselow 1999).Open in a separate windowFigure 1.Experimental design used in Experiments 1 and 2.  相似文献   

6.
Research into the neural mechanisms that underlie higher-order cognitive control of eating behavior suggests that ventral hippocampal (vHC) neurons, which are critical for emotional memory, also inhibit energy intake. We showed previously that optogenetically inhibiting vHC glutamatergic neurons during the early postprandial period, when the memory of the meal would be undergoing consolidation, caused rats to eat their next meal sooner and to eat more during that next meal when the neurons were no longer inhibited. The present research determined whether manipulations known to interfere with synaptic plasticity and memory when given pretraining would increase energy intake when given prior to ingestion. Specifically, we tested the effects of blocking vHC glutamatergic N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors (NMDARs) and activity-regulated cytoskeleton-associated protein (Arc) on sucrose ingestion. The results showed that male rats consumed a larger sucrose meal on days when they were given vHC infusions of the NMDAR antagonist APV or Arc antisense oligodeoxynucleotides than on days when they were given control infusions. The rats did not accommodate for that increase by delaying the onset of their next sucrose meal (i.e., decreased satiety ratio) or by eating less during the next meal. These data suggest that vHC NMDARs and Arc limit meal size and inhibit meal initiation.

Research into the higher-order cognitive controls of eating behavior has demonstrated that hippocampal neurons, which are critical for learning and memory, also regulate energy intake (Benoit et al. 2010; Parent 2016; Kanoski and Grill 2017). The hippocampus is functionally divided along its longitudinal axis into dorsal (posterior in primates) and ventral (anterior in primates) poles (Moser and Moser 1998; Fanselow and Dong 2010; Strange et al. 2014). Generally, dorsal hippocampal (dHC) neurons are necessary for episodic and spatial memory, whereas ventral hippocampal (vHC) neurons are essential for affective and motivational processes and emotional memory (Fanselow and Dong 2010; Strange et al. 2014). dHC and vHC have different anatomical connections, cellular and circuit properties and patterns of gene expression that likely contribute to the different functions that they serve (Moser and Moser 1998; Thompson et al. 2008; Dong et al. 2009; Barkus et al. 2010; Fanselow and Dong 2010; Bienkowski et al. 2018).vHC neurons, in particular, are poised to integrate energy-related signals with mnemonic processes because they contain receptors for numerous food-related signals (Kanoski and Grill 2017) and project to several brain regions critical for food intake (Namura et al. 1994; Cenquizca and Swanson 2006; Radley and Sawchenko 2011; Hsu et al. 2015b). vHC lesions increase food consumption and body mass (Davidson et al. 2009, 2012, 2013), and activation of vHC receptors for gut hormones affects food intake and food-related memory (Kanoski et al. 2011, 2013; Hsu et al. 2015a, 2017, 2018). Additionally, vHC glutamatergic projections to the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis, lateral septum, and prefrontal cortex inhibit energy intake (Sweeney and Yang 2015; Hsu et al. 2017).It is possible that vHC neurons contribute to the representation of the memory of a meal and inhibit subsequent intake. In support, we have shown that vHC neurons inhibit energy intake during the postprandial period. Specifically, optogenetic inhibition of vHC principle glutamatergic neurons given after the end of a sucrose or chow meal, timed to occur when the memory of the meal would be undergoing consolidation, accelerates the onset of the next meal and increases the amount eaten during the next meal when the neurons are no longer inhibited (Hannapel et al. 2019). Inactivation of these neurons given after a saccharin meal also hastens the initiation of the next saccharin meal and increases the size of that next meal, suggesting that vHC inhibition does not increase intake by disrupting the processing of interoceptive visceral signals (Hannapel et al. 2019).If vHC neurons inhibit intake through a process that involves memory, then well-defined molecular events necessary for vHC synaptic plasticity should play a role in controlling meal timing and meal size because synaptic plasticity at hippocampal excitatory synapses is a critical mechanism underlying memory formation (Bailey et al. 2015; Bartsch and Wulff 2015). Activation of glutamatergic N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors (NMDARs) is required for most forms of hippocampal synaptic plasticity (Malenka and Nicoll 1993; Volianskis et al. 2015). NMDAR-dependent increases in intracellular calcium activate proteins and stimulate mRNA synthesis and protein translation that collectively act to increase glutamate AMPA receptor function in the postsynaptic cell, thereby increasing glutamate signaling and synaptic strength (Shanley et al. 2001; Bevilaqua et al. 2005; Herring and Nicoll 2016). Synaptic plasticity in vHC is NMDAR-dependent and vHC NMDARs are often necessary for vHC-dependent memory (Zhang et al. 2001; Xu et al. 2005; Kent et al. 2007; Czerniawski et al. 2012; Portero-Tresserra et al. 2014; Zhu et al. 2014; Clark et al. 2015; Maggio et al. 2015). Of note, feeding-related hormones such as insulin and leptin enhance NMDAR functionality in hippocampal cultured neurons and slices (Liu et al. 1995; Shanley et al. 2001).Hippocampal synaptic plasticity is also dependent on the activation of the immediate early gene (IEG) activity-regulated cytoskeleton-associated protein (Arc). Arc is considered a master regulator of synaptic plasticity (Bramham et al. 2010; Korb and Finkbeiner 2011; Shepherd and Bear 2011). It is downstream from many molecular signaling pathways and is necessary for virtually every type of synaptic plasticity (Bramham et al. 2008; Korb and Finkbeiner 2011; Shepherd and Bear 2011). Learning experiences produce small but significant increases in Arc that are typically maximal within 15 min of the experience, and unlike other IEGs, Arc expression reflects synaptic plasticity rather than neuronal activity (Fletcher et al. 2006; Guzowski et al. 2006; Carpenter-Hyland et al. 2010). vHC Arc is necessary for memory consolidation because disrupting vHC Arc expression with Arc antisense (anti-Arc) oligodeoxynucleotides (ODN) disrupts vHC-dependent memory (Czerniawski et al. 2011, 2012; Chia and Otto 2013). We have shown that sucrose consumption increases vHC Arc expression during the early postprandial period (Hannapel et al. 2017), suggesting that ingestion activates molecular processes required for synaptic plasticity in vHC.Although it is well established that vHC neurons influence energy regulation, it is unknown whether vHC neurons regulate energy intake through a process that requires NMDARs and Arc. In the present experiments, we tested the prediction that disrupting vHC NMDAR activation and Arc expression would increase meal size and decrease the interval between meals. Specifically, NMDAR antagonists or anti-Arc ODNs were infused into the vHC and subsequent intake of sucrose was assessed.  相似文献   

7.
Balancing exploration and anti-predation are fundamental to the fitness and survival of all animal species from early life stages. How these basic survival instincts drive learning remains poorly understood. Here, using a light/dark preference paradigm with well-controlled luminance history and constant visual surrounding in larval zebrafish, we analyzed intra- and intertrial dynamics for two behavioral components, dark avoidance and center avoidance. We uncover that larval zebrafish display short-term learning of dark avoidance with initial sensitization followed by habituation; they also exhibit long-term learning that is sensitive to trial interval length. We further show that such stereotyped learning patterns is stimulus-specific, as they are not observed for center avoidance. Finally, we demonstrate at individual levels that long-term learning is under homeostatic control. Together, our work has established a novel paradigm to understand learning, uncovered sequential sensitization and habituation, and demonstrated stimulus specificity, individuality, as well as dynamicity in learning.

Learning while being exposed to a stimulus (i.e., nonassociative learning) is of great importance in that it triggers intrinsic constructs for subsequent recognition of that stimulus and provides a foundation for associative learning (e.g., learning about relations between stimuli in Pavlovian conditioning and stimuli responses-outcomes in instrumental conditioning). Nonassociative learning precedes associative learning in the evolutionary sequence and involves a broad range of behavioral phenomena including habituation, sensitization, perceptual learning, priming, and recognition memory (Pereira and van der Kooy 2013; Ioannou and Anastassiou-Hadjicharalambous 2018).As the simplest learning form, habituation is defined as the progressively reduced ability of a stimulus to elicit a behavioral response over time (Glanzman 2009; Rankin et al. 2009; Thompson 2009). Such a response reduction is distinguished from sensory adaptation and motor fatigue and is often considered adaptive in that it helps animals to filter out harmless and irrelevant stimuli (Rankin et al. 2009). Since an early study of EEG arousal in cats (Sharpless and Jasper 1956), the habituation phenomenon has been widely documented in invertebrates such as C. elegans (Rankin and Broster 1992; Rose and Rankin 2001; Giles and Rankin 2009; Ardiel et al. 2016) and Aplysia (Glanzman 2009) as well as in vertebrates such as rodents (Bolivar 2009; Salomons et al. 2010; Arbuckle et al. 2015), zebrafish (Best et al. 2008; Wolman et al. 2011; Roberts et al. 2016; Randlett et al. 2019; Pantoja et al. 2020) and humans (Bornstein et al. 1988; Coppola et al. 2013).Accompanying habituation is a process termed sensitization, which in contrast enhances responses to a stimulus over time (Kalivas and Stewart 1991; McSweeney and Murphy 2009; Robinson and Becker 1986). This counterpart of habituation may also be adaptive if it helps animals avoid potentially risky or costly situations (Blumstein 2016; King et al. 2007). Like habituation, sensitization has also been documented in a phylogenetically diverse set of organisms (Cai et al. 2012; Kirshenbaum et al. 2019; Tran and Gerlai 2014; Watkins et al. 2010), suggesting an evolutionarily conserved biological underpinning for both processes. Furthermore, these simple learning forms are observed in various functional outputs of nervous systems ranging from simple reflexes (Blanch et al. 2014; Pantoja et al. 2020; Pinsker et al. 1970; Randlett et al. 2019) to complex cognitive phenotypes (Bolivar 2009; Leussis and Bolivar 2006; Thompson and Spencer 1966) and may represent deeper neurobiological constructs associated with anxiety-memory interplay (Morgan and LeDoux 1995; Ruehle et al. 2012; Sullivan and Gratton 2002). Therefore, understanding basic building blocks of habituation and sensitization is essential to fully understand complex behaviors.Habituation and sensitization have been reported with short-term (intrasession) and long-term (intersession) mechanisms in a number of systems (Rankin et al. 2009; Thompson 2009). Short-term mechanisms sensitize or habituate a response within a session (Meincke et al. 2004; Leussis and Bolivar 2006; Rahn et al. 2013; Byrne and Hawkins 2015). In contrast, long-term mechanisms retain memory formed in previous session and use it to modify behavioral responses in a subsequent session (Rankin et al. 2009).Although both short- and long-term learning and memory have been demonstrated in young larval zebrafish (Wolman et al. 2011; O''Neale et al. 2014; Roberts et al. 2016; Randlett et al. 2019), so far, most paradigms use unnatural stimuli and are designed without integrating sensitization and habituation in the same paradigm. The latter limitation is crucial as the influential dual-process theory, proposed by Groves and Thompson (1970), suggests that the two learning forms interact to yield final behavioral outcomes and therefore assessment of only one process might be confounded by alteration in the other process (Meincke et al. 2004).In this study, we examined stimulus learning in a large population of larval zebrafish using a light/dark preference paradigm over four trials across 2 d. Light/dark preference as a motivated behavior is observed across the animal kingdom (Serra et al. 1999; Bourin and Hascoët 2003; Gong et al. 2010; Lau et al. 2011). Larval zebrafish display distinct motor behaviors that are sensitive to the intensity of both preadapted and current photic stimuli (Burgess and Granato 2007; Burgess et al. 2010; Facciol et al. 2019). In our paradigm with well-controlled luminance history and constant visual surrounding, larval zebrafish generally display dark avoidance and center avoidance (also known as thigmotaxis) with heritable individual variability and are considered fear- or anxiety-related (Steenbergen et al. 2011; Schnörr et al. 2012; Bai et al. 2016; Wagle et al. 2017; Dahlén et al. 2019). From an ethological perspective, the extent of avoidance is likely a readout of the circuitry that balances anti-predation (i.e., avoid the dark and the center) and free exploration (i.e., approach the dark and the center). As described below, we have uncovered stimulus-specific temporal dynamicity of learning (both short term and long term), as well as individual differences in learning that are under homeostatic control.  相似文献   

8.
9.
10.
The brain processes underlying impairing effects of emotional arousal on associative memory were previously attributed to two dissociable routes using high-resolution fMRI of the MTL (Madan et al. 2017). Extrahippocampal MTL regions supporting associative encoding of neutral pairs suggested unitization; conversely, associative encoding of negative pairs involved compensatory hippocampal activity. Here, whole-brain fMRI revealed prefrontal contributions: dmPFC was more involved in hippocampal-dependent negative pair learning and vmPFC in extrahippocampal neutral pair learning. Successful encoding of emotional memory associations may require emotion regulation/conflict resolution (dmPFC), while neutral memory associations may be accomplished by anchoring new information to prior knowledge (vmPFC).

Emotional arousal is well known to enhance memory for individual items (Schümann and Sommer 2018), but can have impairing effects on associative memory, particularly when items cannot be easily unitized and interitem associations have to be formed (Madan et al. 2012; Murray and Kensinger 2013; Bisby and Burgess 2017). The neural substrates of the impairing effect of emotional arousal on associative memory have only begun to be explored (Bisby et al. 2016; Madan et al. 2017). Emotional arousal may disrupt hippocampal functions that are critical to promote binding and thereby lead to reduced associative memory for emotionally arousing items (“disruption hypothesis”) (Bisby et al. 2016). Conversely, encoding of neutral items may engage extrahippocampal medial temporal lobe (MTL) regions, areas we interpreted to promote better incidental unitization of neutral than negative items, leading to a net-decrease in associative memory for negative items (“bypassing hypothesis”) (Madan et al. 2017).Specifically, in our previous high-resolution fMRI study focussing on MTL regions (Madan et al. 2017), extrahippocampal MTL cortex was more active during encoding of neutral than negative picture pairs, showed a subsequent memory effect (SME) for neutral picture pairs, and neutral pair encoding was accompanied by more between-picture saccadic eye movements compared with negative pairs. In line with previous findings of extrahippocampal MTL areas involved in association memory formation of merged or unitized items (Giovanello et al. 2006; Quamme et al. 2007; Diana et al. 2008; Delhaye et al. 2019), we interpreted our fMRI and eye movement findings to suggest better incidental unitization of neutral picture pairs than negative pictures pairs. A behavioral follow-up study confirmed that unitization, that is, imagining the two pictures as one (“interactive imagery”), was indeed rated as higher for neutral than negative pairs, and this advantage in interactive imagery was linked to better associative memory for neutral pairs, lending further support to the bypassing hypothesis (Caplan et al. 2019).What would prevent emotional pairs from being as easily merged as neutral pairs? We observed that during negative pair encoding, each individual picture was fixated longer compared with neutral pictures. These longer fixation durations for negative pictures were related to greater activity during negative than neutral pair encoding in the dorsal amygdala (likely the centromedial group) (Hrybouski et al. 2016), an activation which was functionally coupled to the more ventral amygdala (likely the lateral nucleus) (Hrybouski et al. 2016). This ventral amygdala activation exhibited a subsequent forgetting effect specifically for negative pairs. Given that emotional items attract more attention to themselves and are more likely processed as individual items (Markovic et al. 2014; Mather et al. 2016), we conjectured that this may make pairs of emotional items harder to unitize and to benefit from extrahippocampal unitization-related processes such as interactive imagery. Interestingly, the hippocampus revealed a subsequent memory effect specifically for negative pairs in Madan et al. (2017). We concluded that when sufficiently arousing information precludes extrahippocampal unitization-based encoding, an alternative, compensatory, and effortful relational hippocampus-dependent encoding strategy may be used.Both findings, extrahippocampal MTL encoding for neutral pairs and compensatory hippocampal encoding for negative pairs, raise the question of which cortical areas could be involved in these two dissociable associative encoding processes. Conceivably, successful associative encoding of negative information may require participants to evaluate the emotional content, and regulate emotional arousal/conflict, functions primarily associated with dorso-medial PFC regions (dmPFC; Dixon et al. 2017), the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) (Botvinick 2007), and posterior areas of the ventro-medial PFC (vmPFC) (Yang et al. 2020). To unitize two pictures through interactive imagery, retrieval of semantic memories and prior knowledge regarding the contents of the two pictures is likely helpful. Semantic memory processes have been attributed to the left inferior frontal gyrus (left IFG) (Binder and Desai 2011). The vmPFC (more anterior portions) could also be involved, owing to its role in relating new information during encoding to prior knowledge, that is, a “unitization-like” process (Gilboa and Marlatte 2017; Sommer 2017). Motivated by our previous discovery and interpretation of the two distinct encoding processes in the MTL (Madan et al. 2017), the potential contribution of these cortical areas in neutral and negative association memory was explored here by using a whole-brain scan and overcoming the limitations of our previous high-resolution fMRI sequence focused only on the MTL in Madan et al. (2017).In the current study, we therefore acquired standard-resolution whole-brain fMRI (3 Tesla Siemens Trio scanner, 3-mm thickness, TR 2.21 sec, TE 30 msec) of 22 male participants during exactly the same task as in Madan et al. (2017). Only male participants were recruited because of known sex-dependent lateralization of amygdala activity (Cahill et al. 2004; Mackiewicz et al. 2006), limiting the conclusions of the current study to males. Eye movements were assessed as a proxy for overt attention (EyeLink 1000, SR Research, 17 participants with usable eye-tracking data). In each of three encoding-retrieval cycles, 25 neutral and 25 negative picture pairs were intentionally encoded. Pictures (e.g., objects, scenes, humans, and animals) were selected from the International Affective Picture System (Lang et al. 2008) and the internet, and confirmed to have different valence and arousal through independent raters (details in Madan et al. 2017). Each encoding round was followed by a two-step memory test for each pair: In a judgement of memory (JoM) task one picture served as retrieval cue and volunteers indicated their memory (yes/no) for the other picture of the original pair. Then the same picture was centrally presented again, surrounded by five same-valence pictures (one correct target, four lures) in a five-alternative forced-choice associative recognition test. Participants chose the target picture from the array with an MR-compatible joystick.As in our previous studies, associative recognition was less accurate for negative (NN) (M = 0.47) than neutral (nn) pairs (M = 0.51; t(22) = 2.49, P = 0.02). Subjective memory confidence (JoM) for neutral pairs (M = 0.41) was not significantly different from confidence for negative pairs (M = 0.43; t(22) = 1.19, P = 0.25) (Fig. 1A; Madan et al. 2017; Caplan et al. 2019).Open in a separate windowFigure 1.Behavioral and eye tracking results. (A) Accuracy in the associative recognition task (5-AFC) for all negative (NN) and neutral (nn) pairs. Chance level in the 5-AFC associative recognition task was 1/5 = 0.20. (B) Mean number of saccades between the two pictures of a pair for remembered (Hit) and forgotten (Miss) negative (NN) and neutral (nn) pairs. (C) Mean number of saccades within pictures. Error bars are 95% confidence intervals around the mean, corrected for interindividual differences (Loftus and Mason 1994).Average fixation duration (a proxy for the depth of processing of individual pictures) was longer for negative than neutral pictures (F(1,16) = 9.59, P = 0.007), with no main effect of memory (F(1,16) = 0.11, P = 0.75), nor emotion–memory interaction (F(1,16) = 1.27, P = 0.28). The number of fixations was also higher for negative than neutral pictures (F(1,16) = 5.56, P = 0.03), again with no main effect of memory (F(1) = 1.56, P = 0.23) or interaction (F(1,16) = 0.26, P = 0.61). The number of saccades within each picture (i.e., visual exploration within but not across items, reflecting intraitem processing) was higher for negative than neutral pairs (Fig. 1B; F(1,16) = 33.38, P < 0.001), with no main effect of memory (F(1,16) = 0.02, P = 0.89) nor interaction (F(1,16) = 0.15, P = 0.71). However, the number of saccades between the two pictures of a pair, which may support associative processing, was substantially lower for negative than neutral pairs (Fig. 1C; F(1,16) = 7.67, P = 0.01). Importantly, there were more between-picture saccades for pairs that were later remembered than forgotten, that is, a subsequent memory effect based on between-picture saccades (F(1,16) = 8.43, P = 0.01). This effect did not further interact with emotion (F(1,16) = 2.64, P = 0.12). Thus, association memory success was driven by interitem saccades, and these were reduced in negative trials. Participants spent more attention to individual negative than neutral pictures (fixation duration and number of within-picture saccades), but this was unrelated to association memory success.The fMRI data were preprocessed (slice timing corrected, realigned and unwarped, normalized using DARTEL and smoothed, FWHM = 8 mm) and analyzed using SPM12. First-level models were created with four regressors that modeled the onsets of the 2 (negative and neutral) × 2 (subsequent hits and misses) conditions of interest. Results of all fMRI analyses were considered significant at P < 0.05, family-wise error (FWE) corrected for multiple comparisons across the entire scan volume or within the a priori anatomical regions of interest (ROIs). ROIs for the hippocampus, amygdala and extrahippocampal MTL were reused from our previous study (Madan et al. 2017). The prefrontal ROIs, that is, dmPFC, ACC, vmPFC and left inferior frontal gyrus ROIs, were manually traced on the mean T1 image using ITK-SNAP 3.6.0 (Yushkevich et al. 2006) following schematic drawings based on meta-analyses (Binder and Desai 2011; Dixon et al. 2017; Gilboa and Marlatte 2017).The second-level analyses based on the resulting individual β images and subject as a random factor replicated a well-established network of brain areas involved in negative emotion processing (Spalek et al. 2015): greater activity during processing negative than neutral picture pairs in the amygdala, insula, right inferior frontal gyrus, mid, and anterior cingulate cortex as well as visual areas (Fig. 2A). As in our previous study, we correlated the difference in left amygdala activity with the difference in eye movements for negative minus neutral trials, showing a significant correlation with the number of within-picture saccades (r = 0.50, P = 0.018). Thus, higher left amygdala activity was associated with increased visual search within negative pictures. We conducted a psychophysiological interaction analysis (PPI) using this amygdala region as seed and contrasted functional coupling during successful versus unsuccessful negative with successful versus unsuccessful neutral pair encoding (i.e., the interaction of valence and subsequent memory success). This PPI revealed stronger coupling during successful encoding of negative compared with neutral pairs with a (nonsignificant) cluster in the dmPFC (Z = 3.01, [−12, 38, 26]). Simple effects showed that the amygdala was more strongly coupled with the dmPFC during successful than unsuccessful negative pair encoding (Z = 3.63, [−2, 16, 42]).Open in a separate windowFigure 2.Main effects of emotion—fMRI results. (A) Greater activity during negative than neutral pair processing irrespective of subsequent memory success. (B) Greater activity during neutral than negative pairs processing. t-maps thresholded at P < 0.001 uncorrected for visualization purposes. t-value color-coded.Neutral-pair processing was associated with greater activity than negative-pair processing in the bilateral extrahippocampal MTL cortex, ventral precuneus (vPC), retrosplenial cortex (RSC), middle occipital gyrus, and putamen (Fig. 2B). In addition, we observed a general SME irrespective of valence in the left hippocampus ([−28, −16, −24], Z = 3.49, P = 0.04).An interaction between pair valence and SME with greater neutral than negative SME was observed in vmPFC (Fig. 3A), together with a (nonsignificant) cluster in right MTL cortex ([26, −24, −28], Z = 3.16, P = 0.11). We conducted a PPI using this vmPFC region as seed and contrasted functional coupling during successful vs. unsuccessful neutral with successful vs. unsuccessful negative pair encoding. This PPI revealed stronger coupling during successful encoding of neutral compared with negative pairs in a cluster at the border of the extrahippocampal MTL cortex reaching into the hippocampus ([−20, −18, −26], Z = 4.61, Fig. 3B).Open in a separate windowFigure 3.SME × Emotion interactions and PPIs. (A) Activity in the vmPFC revealed a SME only for neutral but not negative pairs. (B) This region was stronger coupled during neutral than negative pair encoding with a cluster in the border of left MTL cortex/hippocampus. (C,D) Activity in the right hippocampus and dmPFC revealed a SME only for negative pairs. (E) The dmPFC was stronger coupled during negative than neutral pairs encoding with the bilateral hippocampus. t-maps thresholded at P < 0.001 uncorrected for visualization purposes. Error bars are 95% confidence intervals around the mean, corrected for interindividual differences (Loftus and Mason 1994).Conversely, an interaction between pair valence and SME showing a greater negative than neutral SME was observed in the right hippocampal region (Fig. 3C), replicating our previous finding of compensatory hippocampal encoding, and in the insula (Z = 3.7, [38, 2, 8]). Within prefrontal cortex, the dorsal medial prefrontal cortex (dmPFC, Z = 4.14) (Fig. 3D), also showed this effect. Neutral pairs showed a subsequent forgetting effect, that is, greater activity during unsuccessful encoding of neutral pairs, in these regions (Fig. 3 C,D).Similar to the PPI with the vmPFC seed, we conducted a PPI with the dmPFC cluster as seed. This PPI revealed the bilateral hippocampus to be more strongly coupled with the dmPFC during successful negative than neutral pair encoding (Z = 3.98, [−24, −10, −18], Z = 4.71, [30, −14, −29]) (Fig. 3E). The correlational analyses of activity in the dmPFC and vmPFC (valence × encoding success interactions) with the corresponding eye-tracking measures were nonsignificant, possibly due to low reliability of difference measures (Schümann et al. 2020).The current findings, first, replicated the impairing effects of emotional arousal on association memory previously observed in six experiments across four studies (Madan et al. 2012, 2017; Caplan et al. 2019). We built on these previous findings here by identifying cortical, especially prefrontal areas involved in the associative memory advantage for neutral pairs and those involved in the compensatory mechanism for learning negative pairs. In particular, vmPFC activity more strongly supported successful encoding of neutral than negative pairs and during this process, showed stronger coupling with a cluster at the border between MTL cortex and hippocampus. Conversely, the dmPFC was more engaged and more strongly coupled with the hippocampus during successful negative than neutral pair encoding.We observed more and longer fixations, as well as more within-picture saccades for individual negative pictures compared with neutral pictures, resembling previously reported eye movement findings (Bradley et al. 2011; Dietz et al. 2011). We had previously shown that increased attention (fixation duration) to individual negative pictures is linked to centromedial amygdala activity (not measurable here due to the whole-brain scan resolution), and functionally coupled with a negative pair-specific subsequent forgetting effect in the lateral amygdala (Madan et al. 2017). These findings together suggest that increased attention attracted by individual negative pictures does not support associative memory, or may even be detrimental (cf., Hockley and Cristi 1996).The dmPFC contributed more to negative than neutral association memory and was functionally coupled to the hippocampus, which complements our interpretation of possibly compensatory activity in the hippocampus during negative pair encoding (Madan et al. 2017). The amygdala on the other hand was stronger coupled with the dmPFC during successful encoding of negative pairs which might reflect the detection of aversive stimuli by the amygdala. The dmPFC not only plays a role in emotion regulation (Wager et al. 2008; Ochsner et al. 2012; Kohn et al. 2014; Dixon et al. 2017): It is the central node in the cognitive control network. In particular, the dmPFC regulates conflicts between goals and distracting stimuli by boosting attention toward the relevant task (Weissman 2004; Grinband et al. 2011; Ebitz and Platt 2015; Iannaccone et al. 2015). Consistent with this role in the current task, the dmPFC was functionally more strongly coupled with the bilateral hippocampus during successful negative compared with neutral pair learning. The involvement of the dmPFC during successful negative (but unsuccessful neutral) (discussed below) pair encoding may suggest that it resolves conflicts between the prepotent attention to the individual negative pictures and the current task goals, that is, their intentional associative encoding. One way to do so might involve the dmPFC''s role to regulate the negative emotions elicited by the pictures in order to focus on the associative memory task.Neutral pairs elicited more between-picture saccades than negative pairs, as in (Madan et al. 2017). The vmPFC was more strongly involved in successful associative encoding of neutral than negative pairs and more strongly coupled with the extrahippocampal MTL cortex bordering the hippocampus during successful neutral compared with negative pair encoding. Anterior vmPFC regions and their coupling with the MTL have been implicated in retrieval of consolidated memories and in anchoring new information to prior knowledge (Nieuwenhuis and Takashima 2011; van Kesteren et al. 2013; Schlichting and Preston 2015; Gilboa and Marlatte 2017; Sommer 2017; Brod and Shing 2018; Sekeres et al. 2018). We previously observed that interactive imagery (forming one instead of two images to memorize) was higher for neutral than negative pairs (Caplan et al. 2019), perhaps reflected by the increased between-picture saccades in the current study. Assuming that the anterior vmPFC subserves retrieval of prior knowledge, its engagement during successful neutral pair encoding may have supported such incidental unitization processes here as well. Negative pictures are inherently semantically more related (Barnacle et al. 2016), which implies that they may share even more prior knowledge than neutral pictures. However, the retrieval of this prior knowledge may be inhibited by the attraction of attention to individual negative pictures, not their arbitrary pairing as in the current task. Incidental unitization can occur through rather subtle manipulations (Giovanello et al. 2006; Diana et al. 2008; Bader et al. 2010; Ford et al. 2010; Li et al. 2019) or even entirely without any instruction; for example, when the items’ combination is itself meaningful or familiar (Ahmad and Hockley 2014). We suggest that similar incidental unitization processes may have occurred here as well. Memory for unitized associations is independent of hippocampal memory processes and can be based solely on the extrahippocampal MTL (Quamme et al. 2007; Haskins et al. 2008; Staresina and Davachi 2010). Our previous high-resolution fMRI study supported such a bypassing hypothesis, that is, extrahippocampal MTL cortex involvement in the successful associative encoding of neutral but not negative pairs (Madan et al. 2017). Here, this interaction did not reach significance in the MTL cortex, but the P-value of 0.11 can be considered suggestive based on our strong a priori-hypothesis. Notably, in our previous study using a scanning resolution of 1 mm3 the cluster included only 17 voxels, which would correspond to less than one voxel here. Therefore, we assume the lower sensitivity here was due to the lower spatial resolution.Unexpectedly, we observed greater activity during unsuccessful encoding of neutral pairs in the same regions that promoted successful encoding of negative pairs, that is, the dmPFC and hippocampal region. Hockley et al. (2016) previously observed that incidental but not intentional encoding of associations (for word pairs) improved for items with stronger pre-experimental associations. Perhaps using an effortful (dmPFC/hippocampal) learning strategy for neutral pairs, that is, pairs that are already more likely incidentally linked or linkable (e.g., through interactive imagery) may not have helped encoding. The forgotten neutral pairs underlying the SFE in these regions may then have been simply the hardest-to-learn neutral pairs; that is, pairs where both encoding strategies failed. Evidently, future studies should test such speculations directly.Our interpretation of the dmPFC and vmPFC as signifying in emotion regulation and unitization in this task was based on previous studies. Because we did not manipulate unitization and/or emotional regulation, these processes remain hypothetical. However, within this framework, we addressed two hypotheses regarding interactions between hippocampal/extrahippocampal MTL regions and prefrontal cortex during association memory formation. The disruption hypothesis proposes that the hippocampus is equally responsible for encoding of negative and neutral association memory but that for negative memories, hippocampal activity is inhibited by the amygdala via the prefrontal cortex (Murray and Kensinger 2013; Bisby et al. 2016). The vmPFC has known involvement in negative emotion processing (Yang et al. 2020), and the observed activity pattern in the vmPFC could appear to disrupt hippocampal association memory processes for negative pairs. However, according to the bypassing hypothesis (Madan et al. 2017), successful encoding of negative (compared with neutral) pairs requires the hippocampus since fewer extrahippocampal contributions are available. Supporting the bypassing hypothesis, we observed that the vmPFC was negatively functionally coupled with extrahippocampal MTL cortex (bordering the hippocampus), suggesting that the vmPFC decreased extrahippocampal contributions to association memory for negative pairs. The bypassing hypothesis is also supported by our finding that the hippocampus was not less but more involved in negative compared with neutral pair encoding, that is, we observed no evidence for a prefrontally (e.g., vmPFC)-mediated disruption of hippocampal activity by emotion.In conclusion, the two critical prefrontal cortex regions linked to MTL memory processes in the current study were the dmPFC, involved in successful hippocampal-dependent negative pair learning and the vmPFC, supporting successful neutral pair learning that relied on extrahippocampal MTL involvement.  相似文献   

11.
Dysfunctions in memory recall lead to pathological fear; a hallmark of trauma-related disorders, like posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Both, heightened recall of an association between a cue and trauma, as well as impoverished recall that a previously trauma-related cue is no longer a threat, result in a debilitating fear toward the cue. Glucocorticoid-mediated action via the glucocorticoid receptor (GR) influences memory recall. This literature has primarily focused on GRs expressed in neurons or ignored cell-type specific contributions. To ask how GR action in nonneuronal cells influences memory recall, we combined auditory fear conditioning in mice and the knockout of GRs in astrocytes in the prefrontal cortex (PFC), a brain region implicated in memory recall. We found that knocking out GRs in astrocytes of the PFC disrupted memory recall. Specifically, we found that knocking out GRs in astrocytes in the PFC (AstroGRKO) after fear conditioning resulted in higher levels of freezing to the CS+ tone when compared with controls (AstroGRintact). While we did not find any differences in extinction of fear toward the CS+ between these groups, AstroGRKO female but not male mice showed impaired recall of extinction training. These results suggest that GRs in cortical astrocytes contribute to memory recall. These data demonstrate the need to examine GR action in cortical astrocytes to elucidate the basic neurobiology underlying memory recall and potential mechanisms that underlie female-specific biases in the incidence of PTSD.

Recalling important information about salient environmental cues is an integral part of how we navigate our world. Recalling too much, or too little, information about salient environmental cues is a part of the psychopathology of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) (Milad and Quirk 2012). More specifically, the augmented recall of an association between an environmental cue and a traumatic event results in debilitating fear toward the cue, even in the absence of any threat. In contrast, impoverished recall of information that a cue, previously associated with trauma, is no longer a threat also results in debilitating fear toward the cue after it is no longer dangerous. Therefore, one way to mitigate debilitating fear that characterizes PTSD is to understand the neurobiological mechanisms underlying memory recall.Among many mechanisms, glucocorticoid action via signaling through glucocorticoid receptors (GRs) is an important neurobiological pathway that underlies the recall of salient information. When trauma-associated cues are encountered, the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis is activated and GR signaling is consequently triggered (McEwen et al. 1988; McEwen 1992; Lupien et al. 2009). Existing literature demonstrates that glucocorticoids and GRs do in fact influence learning, memory, and the recall of learning (Pugh et al. 1997; de Quervain et al. 1998, 2009, 2011, 2017, 2019; Roozendaal 2002, 2003; Conrad et al. 2004; Hui et al. 2004; Donley et al. 2005; Roozendaal and de Quervain 2005; Cai et al. 2006; Soravia et al. 2006; Yang et al. 2006; Roozendaal et al. 2009; Bentz et al. 2010; Blundell et al. 2011; Clay et al. 2011; Nikzad et al. 2011; Roesler 2012; Liao et al. 2013; Wislowska-Stanek et al. 2013; Arp et al. 2016; Reis et al. 2016; Dadkhah et al. 2018; Inoue et al. 2018; Scheimann et al. 2019; Lin et al. 2020). The relationship between glucocorticoids, GRs, learning and memory is complicated and within the literature cited above, one can find examples of GR action being facilitatory as well as inhibitory to learning and memory recall. As expansive as this research is, the influence of GRs on learning, memory, and recall of learning has mostly focused only on GR action in neurons or has ignored cell type specific contributions. While glia are approximately as common as neurons in the nervous system (von Bartheld et al. 2016; von Bartheld 2018), the role of GRs in glial cells on the recall of salient environmental cues has been neglected. More specifically, while astrocytes comprise a significant proportion of the glial cell population (von Bartheld et al. 2016) and express GRs (Vielkind et al. 1990; Bohn et al. 1991), the influence of GRs in astrocytes on memory recall remains largely unappreciated (for one exception, see the Discussion).Our goal in this study was to determine the influence of GRs in astrocytes on memory recall. To do so, we combined the robust and reliable experimental framework of classical fear conditioning in rodents (Santini et al. 2008; Dias et al. 2014; Bukalo et al. 2015; Keiser et al. 2017; Giustino and Maren 2018; Greiner et al. 2019; Gunduz-Cinar et al. 2019; Venkataraman et al. 2019) with molecular genetic manipulations in the prefrontal cortex (PFC), a brain region critical for the recall of memory (Morgan and LeDoux 1995; Quirk et al. 2000; Mueller et al. 2008; Quirk and Mueller 2008; Giustino and Maren 2015; Rozeske et al. 2015; Maren and Holmes 2016). We first trained mice to associate tone presentations with mild footshocks. After this auditory fear conditioning, we used a CRE-loxP strategy to specifically knock out GRs in astrocytes in the PFC (hereafter termed cortical astrocytes) of these trained mice. We then exposed animals to extinction training: 30 presentations of the tone in the absence of any footshocks. Finally, 1 d after the extinction training, we exposed animals to two presentations of the tone. This experimental timeline allowed us to ask how a lack of GRs in cortical astrocytes influences (1) the recall of the previous aversive association of the tone presentation with the footshock, (2) the extinction of fear that would typically occur during extinction training, and (3) the recall of extinction training allowing us to measure the influence of GRs in cortical astrocytes on the recall of extinction training. Broadly, our results demonstrate that knocking out GRs in cortical astrocytes disrupts fear memory recall in both male and female mice, while only disrupting extinction recall in female mice.  相似文献   

12.
Neocortical sleep spindles have been shown to occur more frequently following a memory task, suggesting that a method to increase spindle activity could improve memory processing. Stimulation of the neocortex can elicit a slow oscillation (SO) and a spindle, but the feasibility of this method to boost SO and spindles over time has not been tested. In rats with implanted neocortical electrodes, stimulation during slow wave sleep significantly increased SO and spindle rates compared to control rest periods before and after the stimulation session. Coordination between hippocampal sharp-wave ripples and spindles also increased. These effects were reproducible across five consecutive days of testing, demonstrating the viability of this method to increase SO and spindles.

Neocortical sleep spindles are prominent oscillations during slow-wave sleep (SWS) (Kandel and Buzsáki 1997; Steriade and Deschenes 1984; Steriade et al. 1993) that play a role in consolidation of both declarative (Gais et al. 2002; Schabus et al. 2004; Schmidt et al. 2006) and nondeclarative memories (Nishida and Walker 2007; Peters et al. 2008; Barakat et al. 2011; Johnson et al. 2012). Spindles are preferred times of memory reactivation in the cortex during sleep, suggesting that reactivation during spindles contributes to memory performance (Peyrache et al. 2009; Ramanathan et al. 2015; Eckert et al. 2020).Based on this evidence, a method to induce spindle oscillations may prove beneficial to memory consolidation. Recent optogenetic experiments have shown that it is possible to induce cortical spindles by activating the thalamic reticular nucleus (Halassa et al. 2011; Kim et al. 2012), and that these induced spindles improve memory performance (Latchoumane et al. 2017). Given the large gap between optogenetic manipulation and clinical practice, a more practical technique for enhancing spindles would be desirable. Electrical stimulation of the neocortex with an implanted electrode can evoke a slow oscillation (SO) and a spindle, but the reliability and longevity of this effect are unknown (Steriade and Deschenes 1984; Contreras and Steriade 1995; Vyazovskiy et al. 2009). Here we show that electrical pulse stimulation reliably evokes spindles for the duration of a 1-h stimulation session, resulting in a significant increase in SO and spindle density.Four adult male Fisher–Brown Norway rats had recording electrodes implanted bilaterally in the motor cortex, hippocampus, and neck. The stimulating electrode was implanted in the deep motor cortex of one hemisphere adjacent to one of the cortical recording electrodes (see Supplemental Fig. S1 for details, as well as the Supplemental Material for greater detail of all methods). After recovering from surgery, the animals were habituated to a quiet, dimly lit room and recording box for 3 d. All procedures and recordings were performed during the animal''s light cycle. The vivarium maintained a 12-h light cycle (7 a.m. on) and the animals were tested in the same order every day such that recordings on different days occurred at approximately the same time (±1 h). On experiment days, rats were taken to the recording room and they rested in a small cage after being connected to the recording system. The recording consisted of three consecutive 1-h sessions: a baseline recording with no stimulation, a recording with repeated single pulse electrical stimulation, and a final recording session with no stimulation. The procedure was repeated on five consecutive days to test the reliability of the method.Recording was done on a Cheetah system (Neuralynx) and all signals were sampled at 2 kHz. Stimulation was delivered by a constant current stimulus isolation unit that was controlled by an Arduino microcontroller. Delivery of stimulation pulses was targeted to SWS using a real-time sleep state detector written in MATLAB. A 3-sec buffer of recording was used to calculate the ratio of δ (1–4 Hz) to θ (5–10 Hz) in the hippocampal LFP as well as the amount of EMG activity. SWS detection occurred when there was a high δ/θ ratio and low EMG activity. For each animal, the threshold value for the SWS detection was determined during a habituation session prior to the start of the stimulation experiment. Once SWS was detected, a single biphasic pulse (150 µS per phase, 500 µA) was delivered every 3 sec for as long as the animal remained in SWS (Fig. 1A). The real-time detection corresponded well with offline sleep structure analysis (Supplemental Fig S3).Open in a separate windowFigure 1.Neocortical stimulation during SWS reliably evokes spindles. (A) Segment of LFP from SWS showing SO and spindle oscillations occurring following stimulation pulses. Colored patches indicate offline detected SO and spindles. (B) Comparison of spontaneous and evoked spindles from each rat. (C) Average waveforms of spontaneous and evoked spindles showing larger amplitude of evoked spindles. Waveforms are aligned to the peak amplitude of the spindle and normalized by the amplitude in the prestim session. Shaded region is SEM of n = 4 rats. (D) Quantification of spindle amplitude across prestim and post-stim recording sessions. Amplitude is normalized to the spindle peak of the prestim session. For the distributions, prestim spindles were used to standardize the amplitude. (*) P < 0.05, t3 = 10.45; (**) P < 0.01, t3 = 5.16. Error bars are SEM of n = 4 rats.Spindles, SO, and SWR were detected automatically based on thresholded power signals of filtered LFP recordings (SO 1–4 Hz, spindle 10–20 Hz, SWR 100–250 Hz) (see the Supplemental Material for details). Threshold values for automated detection of LFP features was done while visually inspecting the prestimulation recording from day 1, and then verifying that they were appropriate by checking the prestimulation session from days 2–5. Once set, the same thresholds were used for all recording sessions on all days. Spindles that occurred within 750 msec of a SO (measured from the peak of the hyperpolarization), SWR, or stimulation pulse were considered “coupled” to the SO/SWR or “evoked” by the stimulation pulse. SO within 500 msec of a stimulation pulse were considered “evoked” (Latchoumane et al. 2017). For triple coupling of SO, spindles, and SWR, the SO was taken as time zero and then both a spindle and SWR had to occur within 750 msec of the SO. Although SOs, spindles and SWR appear similar in mice and rats (Mölle et al. 2009; Niethard et al. 2018; Csernai et al. 2019), differences in detection methods and parameters can give rise to different event timings. Furthermore, definitions of “coupling” can vary across studies, with gaps of 2 sec between SO and spindles accepted as coupled (Kam et al. 2019). Because of these issues, we tested several gap durations where we report significant coupling to ensure the coupling is not specific to a particular gap.As previously reported (Vyazovskiy et al. 2009), brief stimulation pulses delivered to the corpus callosum during SWS evoked SO and spindles in the motor cortex (Fig. 1A). Spindle oscillations occurred reliably following the SO throughout the 1-h stimulation session, an effect that occurred ipsilaterally as well as contralaterally to the stimulation electrode.Other than the presence of a stimulation artifact, which was removed with an automated algorithm (Supplemental Fig. S2), stimulation-evoked spindles appeared qualitatively similar to spontaneously occurring spindles (Fig. 1B). Examination of overlaid average waveforms of spindles from the prestim and stimulation sessions revealed that evoked spindles were larger in amplitude (Fig. 1C). Measuring the peak amplitude showed a significant increase in the amplitude of evoked spindles compared to spontaneous spindles in the prestim session (t3 = 5.16, P < 0.05) (Fig. 1D). This effect did not last beyond the stimulation session, and spindle amplitude in the post-stim session was not significantly different than prestim amplitude (t3 = 0.47, P = 1). We also compared frequency and duration of evoked and spontaneous spindles. Detected spindles were 400–600 msec in duration on average, which is in the range normally reported for spindles (Gardner et al. 2013; Latchoumane et al. 2017), and stimulation did not alter the duration (Supplemental Fig. S4). Similarly, spindle frequency was not changed by stimulation (Supplemental Fig. S4). Like evoked spindles, the amplitude of evoked SO was significantly larger compared to spontaneous SO from the prestim session (Supplemental Fig. S5). Unlike spindles, the amplitude of spontaneous SO in the post-stim session, although smaller than evoked SO, remained significantly larger than the prestim SO (Supplemental Fig. S5).Importantly, stimulation did not affect the rats’ sleep. They spent most of the time sleeping, and the amount of sleep was similar between the stimulation session and the post-stimulation session (Supplemental Fig. S6A). The amount of sleep in the prestim session was less than the other sessions, likely because the rats were still aroused after being transported to the recording room. However, the proportion of SWS was similar between all epochs, indicating that stimulation did not significantly alter the sleep structure (Supplemental Fig. S6B).SO and spindles occurred reliably following stimulation in both hemispheres. On average, 76% of pulses were followed by a SO and 59% were followed by spindles (Supplemental Fig. S7). To quantify the increase in the number of SO and spindles events over the duration of the 1-h stimulation session, we compared the SO and spindle density (number per minute; averaged across hemispheres) of the prestim and post-stim sessions. SO and spindle density both increased significantly during the stimulation session, SO by an average of 30% (range 18%–45%) (Fig. 2A1), and spindles by an average of 37% (range 23%–54%) (Fig. 2B1; individual hemispheres in Supplemental Fig. S8). Temporal coupling of SO and spindles was previously identified as important for memory (Latchoumane et al. 2017). Using a similar measure, we classified spindles as coupled to a SO if they occurred within 750 msec. Despite the significant increase in both SO and spindles, the SO-spindle coupling was not increased by stimulation (Supplemental Fig. S9). Furthermore, stimulation did not appear to cause any lasting effect on SO or spindle density as both SO and spindle density in the post-stim session were comparable to the prestim session. We also calculated the peristimulus time histogram (PSTH) of SO and spindle occurrences and found a significant increase in SO and spindle rate following stimulation pulses (Fig. 2A2,B2). To test the reproducibility and longevity of the stimulation-induced increase in SO and spindles, we repeated the experiment on five consecutive days and observed similar increases in SO and spindle density on each day (Fig. 2C,D).Open in a separate windowFigure 2.One hour of stimulation reliably increases SO and spindle rate. (A1) Average SO density across prestim and post-stim sessions (t3 = 5.19). (A2) Peristimulus time histograms (PSTH) of SO relative to stimulation pulses or randomly distributed pulse times (t3 = 3.86). (B1) Average spindle density across sessions (prestim: t3 = 8.05; stim-post: t3 = 4.88). (B2) PSTH of spindle occurrences relative to stimulation pulses (t3 = 4.71). (C,D) Stimulation-induced increase in SO and spindle rate is consistent across five consecutive days. In all panels, (*) P < 0.05, error bars are SEM of n = 4 rats.We next examined the possible effect of cortical stimulation on hippocampal sharp-wave ripples (SWR). Unlike spindles, stimulation did not affect the overall rate of SWR occurrence (Fig. 3A). However, the timing of SWR was affected by stimulation. SWR were more likely to occur within 300–400 msec following a cortical stimulation pulse, and were less likely to occur at other latencies (ANOVA F(19,114) = 6.7, P < 0.01) (Fig. 3B). The increased tendency of SWR within this time window led to increased coupling between SWR and spindles (t3 = 4.72, P < 0.01) (Fig. 3C). To verify that the increased coupling was not due to the definition of coupling (750-msec gap), we tested a range of gap durations (250–2000 msec) and found the coupling to be robust (Supplemental Fig. S10). The increased SWR-spindle coupling was only present during the stimulation session, and there was a decrease in coupling in the post-stim session such that there was no difference between the prestim and post-stim coupling. Finally, we tested for possible triple coupling of SO, spindles, and SWR. Although triple coupling increased in all four rats during the stimulation session, it failed to reach statistical significance (t3 = 2.10, P = 0.63) (Fig. 3D).Open in a separate windowFigure 3.Effects of cortical stimulation on hippocampal sharp-wave ripples (SWR). (A) The rate of SWR in the prestim and post-stim sessions were similar, indicating that stimulation did not affect the overall rate of SWR. (B) Stimulation altered the timing of SWR. The PSTH of SWR relative to stimulation pulses shows an overall suppression of SWR following the pulse, with a grouping of SWR occurring 300–400 msec after the pulse. (**) P < 0.01, ANOVA F(19,114) = 6.7. (C) Stimulation caused an increase SWR-spindle coupling. Compared to the prestim session, there was a significant increase in the proportion of spindles that occurred within 750 msec of a SWR during the stim session. (**) P < 0.01, t3 = 4.72. The increase in SWR-spindle coupling during the stim session did not persist into the post-stim session. (*) P < 0.05, t3 = 2.49. (D) Triple coupling of SO, spindles, and SWR increased in all four rats during the stimulation session but failed to reach statistical significance (t3 = 2.10, P = 0.063).In summary, our results show that spindles can be evoked reliably by single-pulse electrical stimulation of the neocortex for the duration of a 1-h stimulation session, resulting in a significant increase in SO and spindle density, and an increase in SWR-spindle coupling. One potential limitation of our study is the within animal design. Because the same parameters were used to detect spindles and SO, the observed increase in spindles and SO could be due to an endogenous increase that occurs over time. While we cannot strictly rule out this possibility, it is worth noting that the increase in spindle and SO density decreased significantly in the post-stim session, so the increase cannot be due to a progressive increase in spindles and SO. Together with the very strong cross-correlation of stimulation pulses and spindles/SO, the evidence favors the view that the increase in spindle/SO density was indeed due to the stimulation. Furthermore, this increase is reproducible across five consecutive days of stimulation, suggesting that this method is viable as a deep-brain stimulation implant for increasing SO and spindles.Previous attempts to boost spindle expression during sleep have used different methods with mixed results. Playing auditory stimuli during sleep is a minimally invasive method that has shown promise in altering SWS oscillations. In the context of a TMR experiment, when the auditory stimulus was previously paired with a learning task, tones presented during sleep increased spindles whose content was related to the memory task (Cairney et al. 2018). Even when auditory tones are used in non-TMR experiment (i.e., the tone was not previously paired with a learning task), tones played during sleep increased spindle power and improved memory performance (Ngo et al. 2013). However, a recent study showed a similar increase in spindle power following tone presentation, yet there was no corresponding memory improvement (Henin et al. 2019). Furthermore, the practical use of auditory stimuli outside of a clinical setting has been questioned because it is less reliable and subject to interference from other sounds (Ngo et al. 2015).Initial studies of transcranial electrical stimulation (TES) used mild oscillating currents during SWS and showed that slow oscillations and spindles were increased, and that memory performance was improved (Marshall et al. 2006). Subsequent studies using similar protocols yielded mixed results, and a recent study showed that currents typically used in TES studies are too weak to affect neural activity (Lafon et al. 2017). Our more invasive method of implanting a stimulating electrode in the cortex and delivering brief pulses of current was first shown to be effective at evoking spindles many years ago in anesthetized cats (Roy et al. 1984; Steriade and Deschenes 1984; Contreras and Steriade 1995, 1996). Since then, an increase in spindle power has been observed following electrical stimulation of the neocortex in sleeping rats (Vyazovskiy et al. 2009), or by transcranial magnetic stimulation in humans (Bergmann et al. 2012; Massimini et al. 2007). Despite these results, it has not been shown that repeated cortical stimulation is capable of increasing spindle rates, and our current results show that stimulation pulses through implanted electrodes can be used to evoke spindles reliably, not only during a 1-h recording session, but across multiple days.A recent optogenetic study showed that thalamic reticular activation increased the coupling of spindles and slow oscillations and improved contextual fear memory (Latchoumane et al. 2017). Interestingly, they did not report a net increase in spindle density, so the improved memory was attributed to the increased coordination between SO, spindles, and SWR. We observed a significant increase in SWR-spindle coupling, as well as a strong trend toward increased triple coupling of SO, spindles, and SWR, although this failed to reach statistical significance. The weaker triple coupling is possibly due to the fact that our stimulation pulses occurred randomly with respect to SO, whereas the reticular stimulation in Latchoumane et al. (2017) was triggered by a SO. If our electrical stimulation was triggered by SO, then it is possible the triple coupling would be increased as well, although this remains to be tested. Another critical next step is to determine if the substantial stimulation-induced increase spindle and SO density, as well as the increased SWR-spindle coupling, is sufficient to improve memory.  相似文献   

13.
Awake quiescence immediately after encoding is conducive to episodic memory consolidation. Retrieval can render episodic memories labile again, but reconsolidation can modify and restrengthen them. It remained unknown whether awake quiescence after retrieval supports episodic memory reconsolidation. We sought to examine this question via an object-location memory paradigm. We failed to probe the effect of quiescence on reconsolidation, but we did observe an unforeseen “delayed” effect of quiescence on consolidation. Our findings reveal that the beneficial effect of quiescence on episodic memory consolidation is not restricted to immediately following encoding but can be achieved at a delayed stage and even following a period of task engagement.

For labile new memories to be remembered they must be consolidated, that is, strengthened and stabilized, over time (Dudai 2004; Wixted 2004). Extensive research demonstrates that postencoding sleep and awake quiescence (quiet rest) are conducive to human episodic memory consolidation (Clemens et al. 2005; Ferrara et al. 2008; Lahl et al. 2008; Wamsley et al. 2010; Dewar et al. 2012, 2014; Gaskell et al. 2014; Mercer 2015; Brokaw et al. 2016; Craig and Dewar 2018; Craig et al. 2019; Sacripante et al. 2019). Sleep and wakeful rest are hypothesized to support consolidation by providing a state of reduced sensory input and task engagement (Hasselmo 1999; Wixted 2004; Mednick et al. 2011; Dewar et al. 2012, 2014; Craig and Dewar 2018; Craig et al. 2019).Memories are not destined to remain fixed and unmodifiable once consolidated. An increasing body of evidence suggests that the cueing or retrieval of consolidated memories, for example, via internally or externally generated exposure to learned materials (Wichert et al. 2011; Agren 2014), can return such memories into a labile state (Dudai and Eisenberg 2004; McKenzie and Eichenbaum 2011; Schwabe et al. 2014). These relabilized memories are thought to require a process of “reconsolidation” (Rodriguez et al. 1993; Przybyslawski and Sara 1997) in order to restabilize and persist. Evidence for reconsolidation in humans comes from research demonstrating that memories can be disrupted and/or updated by the application of treatments shortly following their retrieval. This includes (1) the disruption and modification of episodic memories via the presentation of similar/related materials (Loftus 2005; Hupbach et al. 2007), (2) a reduction in the number of traumatic memory intrusions following postretrieval engagement in visuospatial tasks (Deeprose et al. 2012; James et al. 2015; Hagenaars et al. 2017), and (3) the extinction of fear memories and increased forgetting of emotional memories via the administration of pharmaceuticals and electrophysiological stimulation (Cahill et al. 1994; Debiec and Ledoux 2004; Kroes et al. 2014). In all cases, the effect of postretrieval treatment is seen only for memories that are retrieved (e.g., via a memory test or cue) prior to the treatment. Memories that were not retrieved (i.e., controls) were unaffected, indicating that the treatment hampered reconsolidation specifically.Episodic memory reconsolidation can also be enhanced. Of particular interest to our study is the finding that reconsolidation, like consolidation, benefits from sleep. If recently or remotely encoded episodic memories are reactivated before an extended period of sleep, the reactivated memories are protected against further forgetting (for reviews, see Stickgold and Walker 2007; Dudai 2012; Spiers and Bendor 2014). Moreover, even naps benefit reconsolidation. It has been shown that a short 40-min nap period facilitated the reconsolidation of remote memories, whereas recently encoded memories did not benefit at all (Klinzing et al. 2016). It is possible that, as is the case for consolidation, sleep is conducive to reconsolidation because it provides a state of reduced cognitive engagement and sensory input (Hasselmo 1999; Wixted 2004; Mednick et al. 2011; Dewar et al. 2012). If so, awake quiescence, which has this in common with sleep, should also be conducive to reconsolidation.In the study reported here, we examined whether a period of awake quiescence following retrieval protects episodic memories from further forgetting by facilitating their reconsolidation. To this end, we combined a computerized item-location memory test with a modified version of our established consolidation paradigm, which compares between-subject effects of postencoding rest versus task engagement on recently encoded memories (e.g., Dewar et al. 2012). Item-location memory tests have previously been sensitive to the effects of postretrieval (reconsolidation) manipulations, including sleep (e.g., Klinzing et al. 2016) hence, subtle differences in memory scores can be recorded. Figure 1 provides an overview of the procedure. Sixty young adults (mean age = 21.47 yr, SD = 1.79 yr; 26M:34F) were sequentially presented photos of 60 everyday items in unique locations on a 22-in touchscreen computer monitor. Immediately after each object''s presentation, participants were asked to recall the object''s location via a touchscreen response as part of a “short-term spatial memory test” (encoding). They were unaware that they would perform subsequent long-term memory tests pertaining to the locations of these items. Location memory for half (N = 30) of the encoded items (“retrieved items”) was probed in a first delayed recall test that occurred after a 10-min delay filled with a visual spot-the-difference game (Dewar et al. 2012). There was no overlap between encoded items and spot-the-difference game stimuli. The first delayed recall test was followed by one of two postretrieval 10-min delay periods: (1) awake quiescence (quiet resting under strict conditions of minimal sensory input in a dimly lit room; N = 30 participants), or (2) a further filler task (a spot-the-difference game comprising different stimuli to earlier; N = 30 participants) (Dewar et al. 2012). Memory for the location of all 60 encoded items was then probed via a second delayed recall test. This test included the 30 items that were probed during the first delayed recall test (retrieved items) and the 30 control items that were not probed during the first delayed recall test (nonretrieved items). Our key measure was the distance of error (in centimeters) between the initially presented locations of the everyday objects during encoding, and the recalled location during the first and second delayed recall tests. We also recorded the time that participants took to respond as well as confidence ratings during encoding and the first and second delayed recall tests. Analyzes were performed using SPSS 19 using ANOVAs, RM ANOVAs. and follow up t-tests that were used to investigate possible between-group differences. See the Supplemental Materials for detailed methods.Open in a separate windowFigure 1.Experimental paradigm. Participants were presented 60 photos of unique everyday items from the Mnemonic Similarity Task (e.g., Stark et al. 2013) in unique locations on a computer screen. They then experienced 10 min of a postencoding delay task (a spot-the-difference game). Memory for the location of half of the encoded items (N = 30) was then probed via a first delayed recall test (via touchscreen response), before participants completed one of two postretrieval delay periods, each of which was 10 min in duration: awake quiescence (N = 30) (A), or an engaging perceptual task (a further spot-the-difference game comprising different stimuli to the earlier one) (N = 30) (B). In the subsequent second delayed recall test, participants’ memory for the location of all the retrieved and nonretrieved items was probed (N = 60) (via touchscreen response). The experimental procedure took place in a single session.Given that our manipulation occurred during the postretrieval delay, we expected groups to be matched in their encoding, postencoding delay task, and first delayed memory test. Crucially, if postretrieval awake quiescence is conducive to episodic memory reconsolidation, then those who experience awake quiescence after the first delayed recall test should demonstrate less forgetting of “retrieved” items (smaller distances of error scores) in the second delayed recall test relative to those who experience the filler task after the first delayed recall test. Given that “control” items were not retrieved during the first delayed recall test immediately prior to our experimental manipulation (postretrieval quiescence vs. task), we predicted that memory (distance of error scores) for control items should be matched between groups in the second delayed recall test.Data from two participants (perceptual task group) were lost due to technical issues. Thus, the following analyzes report data from 58 participants (quiescence group: N = 30, perceptual task group: N = 28). The two groups were well matched in their backgrounds and performance of the postencoding delay task (spot-the-difference game) (see the Supplemental Material). Moreover, as expected, the two groups were well matched in all memory measures occurring prior to our experimental manipulation. Specifically, we found no significant main effect of group in the mean distance of error (in centimeters) during (1) encoding of the items (quiescence: mean = 1.84 cm, SD = 0.57; perceptual task: mean = 2.00 cm, SD = 0.47; F(1,56) = 1.303, P = .259, η2ρ = 0.023), (2) the first delayed recall test of the 30 “retrieved” items (quiescence: mean = 13.36 cm, SD = 3.37; perceptual task: mean = 14.17 cm, SD = 3.67; F(1,56) = = 0.775, P = 0.383, η2ρ = 0.014), or (3) the response times and confidence ratings in the first delayed recall test (see the Supplemental Material).The second delayed recall test was completed immediately following our experimental manipulation, where participants experienced 10 min of either (1) awake quiescence (N = 30 participants), or (2) ongoing sensory input and cognitive engagement via an unrelated perceptual task (a different spot-the-difference game; Dewar et al. 2012) (N = 30 participants). This delayed recall test included the 30 items that were probed during the first delayed recall test (retrieved items) and the 30 control items that were not probed during the first delayed recall test (nonretrieved items). A RM ANOVA comprised of within-subject factor item type (retrieved vs. nonretrieved) and between-subject factor group (awake quiescence vs. perceptual task) revealed no significant main effect of group in distance of error scores (in centimeters; F(1,56) = 0.686, P = 0.411, η2ρ = 0.012). We did, however, find a significant main effect of item type (retrieved vs. nonretrieved; F(1,56) = 14.138, P < 0.001, η2ρ = 0.202) because, overall, the distance of error (in centimeters) was larger for nonretrieved (mean = 14.56 cm, SD = 3.49 cm) than retrieved (mean = 13.36 cm, SD = 3.38 cm) items. There was a significant interaction between group (awake quiescence vs. perceptual task) and item type (retrieved vs. nonretrieved; F(1,56) = 4.561, P = 0.037, η2ρ = 0.075).Paired t-tests revealed that the above interaction emerged because, in the perceptual task group, the distance of error was significantly larger for nonretrieved (mean = 15.27, SD = 3.32 cm) than retrieved (mean = 13.37 cm, SD = 3.64 cm) items (t(27) = −5.05, P < 0.001; see Fig. 2). This significant finding survived Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons (α level of 0.050/four comparisons = corrected α level of 0.013). This was not the case in the quiescence group, where no significant item type difference was observed (retrieved: mean = 13.36 cm, SD = 3.49 cm; nonretrieved: mean = 13.89 cm, SD = 3.26 cm; t(29) = −1.018, P = 0.317). Independent t-tests revealed no effect of delay group in the distance of error for retrieved items (quiescence: mean = 13.36 cm, SD = 3.49 cm; task: mean = 13.37, SD = 3.33; t(56) = −0.008, P = 0.994) or nonretrieved items (quiescence: mean = 13.88 cm, SD = 3.26 cm; task: mean = 15.27 cm, SD = 3.64 cm; t(56) = −1.530, P = 0.132). The groups were matched in their second delayed recall test response times and confidence ratings (see the Supplemental Material).Open in a separate windowFigure 2.Memory performance in the second delayed recall test. Mean distance of error (in centimeters) in the second delayed recall test for items that were and were not “retrieved,” that is, probed, during the first delayed recall test. The data for both the quiescence and task groups are shown. Distance of error refers to the deviation in location from the initially presented location of each item. Error bars show the standard error of the mean. Full lines show between-group comparisons (quiescence vs. task) of distance of error (in centimeters) scores, and dashed lines show within-subject comparisons (retrieved vs. nonretrieved).Our data reveal comparable delayed recall performance for recently retrieved item locations in participants who rested quietly after retrieval and those who played a spot-the-difference game after retrieval (see the “retrieved” data in Fig. 2). Although these findings could suggest that awake quiescence had no effect on the reconsolidation of item-location memories, methodological shortfalls (discussed below) render this conclusion unlikely. However, we did find an unforeseen beneficial effect of quiescence for nonretrieved versus retrieved photo locations: Those who experienced the spot-the-difference game in the second delay phase demonstrated significant forgetting of nonretrieved items relative to retrieved items, whereas those who experienced awake quiescence in the second delay phase did not demonstrate such forgetting (see the “nonretrieved” data in Fig. 2). We discuss these two key findings in turn.Why did we not observe any effects of postretrieval awake quiescence on reconsolidation in our study? One possibility is that, unlike sleep, awake quiescence simply does not benefit reconsolidation. A much more likely possibility is that methodological shortfalls mean that our study did not in fact probe reconsolidation. This may be true for a few reasons. First, sometimes retrieval is not sufficient to reactivate memories and reintroduce them into a labile state (e.g., Cammarota et al. 2004; Forcato et al. 2009). In fact, the protective effect of retrieval against forgetting is well documented, even over a delay of a few minutes (Karpicke and Roediger 2008; Roediger and Butler 2011), possibly because it provides a “fast route to consolidation” of new memories (Antony et al. 2017). Notwithstanding the power of retrieval to boost memory, the research discussed earlier shows that memories reactivated via retrieval can (still) benefit from postretrieval sleep. Do these studies differ from our own with regards to memory reactivation? Possibly, reactivation in our study occurred after a short delay (10 min). It is possible that retrieval was so effective at protecting against forgetting in our study because it occurred shortly after encoding and during the initial consolidation of memory traces. Indeed, studies investigating the role of different treatments (e.g., sleep, pharmaceuticals) in reconsolidation typically require participants to retrieve previously encoded memories after a much longer delay period (several hours to days). After lengthy delays such as those, encoded memories would be expected to have completed initial consolidation, or at least be much further along in the consolidation process than in our study. The 10-min delay in our study might simply have been too short for memories to consolidate sufficiently to be relabilized again. As a result, retrieval might have simply strengthened memories against forgetting and did not sufficiently return them to a labile state. These methodological shortfalls mean that our study is unlikely to have probed reconsolidation effectively. Therefore, we cannot conclude whether postretrieval awake quiescence benefits the reconsolidation of episodic memories.How can we explain the unforeseen finding that “postretrieval” awake quiescence protected against subsequent forgetting for nonretrieved (control) items, which were not probed in the first delayed recall test? Our data reveal an effect of delayed rest that was on par with the overall beneficial effect of retrieval-mediated memory reactivation (i.e., the well-established effect of retrieval practice; see above). The findings suggest that in the task group, the lack of reactivation for nonretrieved items in the first delayed recall test negatively affected these memories. However, in the quiescence group, this lack of reactivation was offset by delayed rest, which seemingly had a similarly beneficial effect on nonretrieved memories. It is unlikely that this benefit of delayed rest can be explained by intentional rehearsal. The occurrence of quiet rest after a 10-min filled delay, as opposed to immediately after encoding, makes it unlikely that participants rehearsed the nonretrieved items during the rest period because they would require explicit knowledge that they had been tested on only half of the encoded items in the first delayed recall test and spend their time rehearsing what they could recall from the subset of items that were not tested. While this does not rule out rehearsal completely, our findings reinforce recent data (e.g., Dewar et al. 2014) showing that rest-related enhancements in memory cannot simply be explained by intentional rehearsal. A consolidation account can provide a more likely explanation.Previous research on the beneficial effect of awake quiescence on memory has focused on immediate rest (i.e., rest periods occurring immediately following encoding) (Dewar et al. 2007, 2012, 2014; Craig et al. 2015, 2016; Mercer 2015; Brokaw et al. 2016; Craig and Dewar 2018; Humiston and Wamsley 2018; Sacripante et al. 2019). The rationale for this focus has been that new memories are most labile upon their initial formation (Mednick et al. 2011; Wixted and Cai 2013), and thus, supportive or disruptive interventions applied during this time should be most effective. The findings of the current study suggest that the effect of awake quiescence is sufficiently powerful to influence new memories even 10–20 min after encoding, that is, when memories have—presumably—benefited from some initial consolidation and are no longer highly labile. Moreover, they indicate that awake quiescence can support the consolidation of new memories even if the awake quiescence occurs after a period of task engagement rather than immediately after encoding. The latter hypothesis resonates with the view that consolidation is an opportunistic process (Mednick et al. 2011) and raises interesting questions about how late the onset of rest would need to be for it to no longer facilitate the consolidation of new memories. This remains to be established, but we predict that the benefit of quiescence should diminish in accordance with increasing delay in the onset of the rest period as memories are increasingly stabilized through consolidation that occurs independently of that during intentional rest. Investigation of this question may provide insights into the initial timeline of episodic memory consolidation, which is characterized poorly. Thus, more work is required to establish whether a relationship exists between the precision of item location memory and the time lag between initial encoding and postencoding quiescence.In summary, methodological shortfalls mean that we failed to probe the possible effect of awake quiescence on episodic memory reconsolidation. Nonetheless, we unexpectedly revealed that awake quiescence following encoding reduces forgetting, even when experienced at a delayed point in time and following a period of task engagement. This finding indicates that awake quiescence need not commence immediately following encoding to benefit memory consolidation, at least for object location memories. Further investigation and independent replication of this effect are required to better understand the underlying mechanisms and implications of our unexpected finding.  相似文献   

14.
Sleep following learning facilitates the consolidation of memories. This effect has often been attributed to sleep-specific factors, such as the presence of sleep spindles or slow waves in the electroencephalogram (EEG). However, recent studies suggest that simply resting quietly while awake could confer a similar memory benefit. In the current study, we examined the effects of sleep, quiet rest, and active wakefulness on the consolidation of declarative and procedural memory. We hypothesized that sleep and eyes-closed quiet rest would both benefit memory compared with a period of active wakefulness. After completing a declarative and a procedural memory task, participants began a 30-min retention period with PSG (polysomnographic) monitoring, in which they either slept (n = 24), quietly rested with their eyes closed (n = 22), or completed a distractor task (n = 29). Following the retention period, participants were again tested on their memory for the two learning tasks. As hypothesized, sleep and quiet rest both led to better performance on the declarative and procedural memory tasks than did the distractor task. Moreover, the performance advantages conferred by rest were indistinguishable from those of sleep. These data suggest that neurobiology specific to sleep might not be necessary to induce the consolidation of memory, at least across very short retention intervals. Instead, offline memory consolidation may function opportunistically, occurring during either sleep or stimulus-free rest, provided a favorable neurobiological milieu and sufficient reduction of new encoding.

Of the myriad new experiences we encode each day, only a fraction are remembered over the long-term. The formation of long-term memory is crucial for optimal functioning in our everyday lives and for building knowledge across days, weeks, and years. Such enduring memories require not only the effective encoding of new information, but also a set of postencoding processes, termed “consolidation,” that function to stabilize and transform new memory traces over time (McGaugh 2000; Frankland and Bontempi 2005; Genzel and Wixted 2017).Consolidation of memory is better supported by some states of consciousness than others. For example, sleep has long been known to optimize memory consolidation, purportedly due to specific neurobiology that actively promotes the consolidation process (Diekelmann and Born 2010). Numerous studies have demonstrated that sleep facilitates the consolidation of both declarative and procedural memories. Slow oscillations (Huber et al. 2004; Marshall et al. 2006) and slow wave sleep (SWS) (Alger et al. 2012; Diekelmann et al. 2012) are thought to especially benefit hippocampus-dependent, declarative memory. Meanwhile, various forms of implicit and procedural memory have been linked to rapid eye movement (REM) sleep (Plihal and Born 1997; Mednick et al. 2009) or non-REM stage 2 (N2) sleep (Walker et al. 2002; Tucker and Fishbein 2009).A potential mechanism of offline memory consolidation during sleep is memory “reactivation,” in which patterns of neural activity in the hippocampus and cortex associated with awake experience are reiterated after learning. For example, when rats sleep after being trained on a spatial learning task, hippocampal “place cells” fire again in the same order as when the animals were being trained on the task during wake (Lee and Wilson 2002; Ji and Wilson 2007). Such neural reactivation not only occurs in the hippocampus, but also concurrently in a variety of cortical areas (Ji and Wilson 2007; Peyrache et al. 2009; Kaefer et al. 2020). The recent advent of optogenetics has allowed experimental investigation of memory reactivation in animal models. Experimentally disrupting the hippocampal ripple oscillations during which reactivation occurs impairs memory (Girardeau et al. 2009; Ego-Stengel and Wilson 2010). Conversely, selectively reactivating neural ensembles related to a particular memory appears to induce consolidation, particularly when this manipulation is applied during sleep or light amnesia (de Sousa et al. 2019).But is sleep the only brain state that facilitates memory consolidation in this way? It has been argued that sleep-specific neurobiology, including sleep slow waves (Alger et al. 2012), sleep spindles (Wamsley et al. 2012; Mednick et al. 2013; Laventure et al. 2016), and/or REM sleep (Karni et al. 1994; Stickgold et al. 2000; McDevitt et al. 2015; Boyce et al. 2016), is required for offline memory reactivation and consolidation to occur, or at least to occur optimally. However, a growing body of literature indicates that stimulus-free waking rest can similarly facilitate consolidation (Wamsley 2019). In two influential experiments, Dewar et al. (2012) demonstrated that compared with participants who completed a nonverbal distractor task, those who rested quietly with their eyes closed in a darkened room for 10 min after learning showed better memory for short stories encoded prior to the retention period. The rest group significantly outperformed the wake group after 15 min, 30 min, and 7 d (experiment 1), even in the absence of retrieval practice during the 7-d period (experiment 2).This effect of post-training rest on memory retention has been reported in an increasing number of papers across the last decade (Gottselig et al. 2004; Mercer 2015; Martini et al. 2018; Wamsley 2019; Martini and Sachse 2020). Brokaw et al. (2016) replicated the behavioral observations of Dewar et al. (2012) and showed that this memory benefit was associated with EEG slow oscillation activity, which is thought to facilitate hippocampal–cortical communication and concomitant memory consolidation during sleep (Marshall et al. 2006; Mölle and Born 2011). A recent study by Sattari et al. (2019) also linked improved memory performance to waking EEG slow oscillations, suggesting that the memory-enhancing effects of rest and sleep may share a common mechanism.Of course, it has been known for decades that at least some consolidation must occur during wakefulness. Local, cellular level consolidation begins to stabilize memory immediately following encoding (Bailey and Kandel 2008; Redondo and Morris 2011), enabling us to recall the events of the previous hours in the absence of intervening sleep. The novel suggestion of these more recent studies is that consolidation does not occur equivalently during all types of wakefulness (Dewar et al. 2012; Brokaw et al. 2016). Instead, stimulus-free rest periods appear to have features that are especially suited to facilitate memory.Reduced sensory processing during eyes-closed rest may be one factor accounting for the memory facilitation effect. However, even internally generated stimuli can also function to block consolidation, as demonstrated by the fact that mental tasks such as retrieval of autobiographical memory and focused meditation are also associated with a reduction in rest''s memory benefit (Craig et al. 2014; Collins and Wamsley 2020). Similarly, we reported in two previous studies that individuals with a high propensity for daydreaming show less memory benefit following a period of rest, presumably because intense internally generated mental activity inhibits consolidation (Humiston et al. 2019; Wamsley and Summer 2020).Together, these observations suggest that consolidation occurs during wakefulness when sensory processing is reduced, when low-frequency EEG oscillations are increased, and when internally generated cognition is at a minimum. Therefore, consolidation may not depend on neural mechanisms specific to sleep, instead opportunistically occurring across multiple states of consciousness whenever the correct conditions are met (Mednick et al. 2011). According to the opportunistic theory of memory consolidation, the processes of encoding and consolidation are mutually exclusive: During any brain state in which we are not currently encoding new information, existing memories consolidate as the neural milieu becomes favorable (Mednick et al. 2011; Wamsley 2019). Unoccupied quiet rest, like sleep, is a state in which the encoding of new stimuli is reduced. In addition, quiet rest and sleep also share a number of neurobiological features that are thought to actively promote memory consolidation, including overall slower EEG in comparison with active wakefulness, increased activation of default-mode network brain structures (Buckner and Vincent 2007), and decreased levels of acetylcholine in the brain (Hasselmo and McGaughy 2004). Additionally, the cellular-level offline reactivation of memory occurs not only during sleep, but also during quiet rest (Foster and Wilson 2006; Karlsson and Frank 2009; Carr et al. 2011; Staresina et al. 2013). At the same time, it must be noted that eyes-closed rest does not replicate all aspects of sleep neurobiology proposed to facilitate memory. For example, sleep spindle oscillations and sleep-specific neurohormonal changes are not present during eyes-closed rest.While sleep and rest both benefit memory in comparison with active wakefulness, it is not known whether they do so equivalently. With few studies directly comparing the size of rest''s memory benefit with that of sleep, it remains possible that sleep provides some benefit above and beyond that conferred by waking rest. The limited number of prior studies in this area have shown mixed results. As opposed to the type of truly task-free condition used in the waking rest studies reviewed above, most experiments comparing active wakefulness, rest, and sleep have used a “rest” condition in which participants are asked to complete an undemanding activity such as listening to music or books on tape. This approach sacrifices complete sensory restriction in return for allowing rest condition participants to maintain wakefulness for longer periods of time. For example, Mednick and colleagues have used quiet rest conditions in which participants listen to music or audiobooks, finding that this form of quiet rest facilitates memory equivalently to sleep for some forms of learning (Mednick et al. 2009; Sattari et al. 2019), but not for others (Mednick et al. 2002; McDevitt et al. 2014). Keeping rest participants awake via verbal instructions to alternately open and close their eyes, Simor et al. (2018) found no effect of post-training rest or sleep on performance of a serial reaction time task, relative to an active wake control. While these observations might indicate that only selected forms of memory can be consolidated during resting wakefulness, it is possible that the encoding of meaningful auditory stimuli during these rest conditions prevents optimal consolidation.Only a few prior studies have compared the memory effects of sleep with those of an equivalent duration of eyes-closed, entirely task-free rest. For example, Gottselig et al. (2004) successfully compared the effects of sleep, quiet rest, and active wakefulness on memory consolidation using a carefully controlled rest condition without any stimuli presented to the participants. Using a statistical auditory sequence learning task, they reported that both sleep and rest equivalently facilitated retention. In contrast, using a declarative memory task, Piosczyk et al. (2013) found that neither quiet rest nor sleep improved memory more than active wakefulness. A recent study from our own laboratory directly compared sleep, rest, and active wakefulness using declarative and procedural tasks commonly used in studies of sleep and memory (Tucker et al. 2020). However, high rates of attrition (due to rest participants inadvertently falling asleep and sleep participants failing to obtain sleep) prevented a robust test of our hypothesis that sleep and quiet rest would equivalently benefit memory, relative to active wakefulness.The goal of the current study was to directly compare the memory benefit of a brief nap (<30 min) with that of an equivalent duration of task-free quiet rest. We hypothesized that a brief period of sleep and quiet rest would have an equivalent effect on the consolidation of both declarative and procedural memories, significantly boosting memory retention compared with an equivalent duration of active wakefulness. Following our previous work, we expected that memory retention across sleep and quiet rest would be associated with slow oscillation EEG power in the <1-Hz range, but that participants with high trait daydreaming propensity would show less improvement across quiet rest.  相似文献   

15.
The depolarization is also important for the short-term synaptic plasticity, known as depolarization-induced suppression of excitation (DSE). The two major types of neurons and their synapses in the lateral nucleus of amygdala (LA) are prone to plasticity. However, DSE in interneurons has not been reported in amygdala in general and in LA in particular. Therefore, we conducted the patch-clamp experiments with LA interneurons. These neurons were identified by lack of adaptation in firing rate of action potentials. In this study, we show for the first time a transient suppression of neurotransmission at synapses both within the local network and between cortical inputs and interneurons of the LA. The retrograde neurotransmission from GABAergic interneurons were comparable with that of glutamatergic pyramidal cells. That is the axonal terminals of cortical inputs do not posses selectivity toward two neuronal subtypes. However, the DSE of both types of neurons involve an increase in intracellular Ca2+ and the release of endogenous cannabinoids (eCB) and activation of presynaptic CB1 receptors. The magnitude of DSE was significantly higher in interneurons compared with pyramidal cells, though developed with some latency.

…I made experiments on myself and my assistant, using smaller doses, and not repeating them so often… Clouston 1870
The biological actions of endogenous cannabinoids (eCB) occur by binding to the CB1 and CB2 receptors throughout the whole body (Ameri 1999; Pertwee 2006; Hill et al. 2007; Yoshida et al. 2011). The density of CB1 receptors in the amygdala is comparably high in mammals (Herkenham et al. 1990).Amygdala similar to hippocampus is important for memory formation and often studied to elucidate plasticity at cellular level using the classical paradigm of Pavlov that continuously serves as a substrate (Pavlov 1927; Bliss and Lomo 1973; Rogan et al. 1997). The amygdala not only receives, but also sends behavior underlying signals into other regions (Racine et al. 1983; Aggleton and Mishkin 1984). While the role of hippocampus is crucial for memory formation, those associated with many different kinds of emotions are mainly modulated by the amygdala (Bucherelli et al. 2006; Fujii et al. 2020). The memory enabling substrate is a long-term potentiation (LTP) of neurotransmission into the postsynaptic neurons (Rogan et al. 1997; Kodirov et al. 2006).The short-term synaptic plasticity in the form of depolarization-induced suppression of either excitation or inhibition (DSE and DSI) has been reported in several regions of the brain (Alger et al. 1996; Kano et al. 2009; Ivanova and Storozhuk 2011). We have discovered DSE in the lateral amygdala (LA), specifically at cortical inputs into the pyramidal neurons (Kodirov et al. 2010).Despite the extensive studies on DSE and DSI, there are only three papers on interneurons that we are aware of. Two of them describe the presence of these phenomena: One study was carried out on parvalbumin immunoreactive interneurons of the stratum radiatum in the hippocampus (Ali 2007) and another on cerebellar stellate and basket cells (Beierlein and Regehr 2006). However, none of the cortical interneurons exhibited DSI despite the presence of a functional and cannabinoid-sensitive inhibitory inputs (Lemtiri-Chlieh and Levine 2007). The retrograde neurotransmission (Llano et al. 1991) takes place via the release of two natural ligands of endogenous cannabinoids anandamide and 2-arachidonoyl-glycerol (Urbanski et al. 2009). These ligands also suppress the evoked excitatory neurotransmission when applied exogenously in vitro (Ameri et al. 1999; Ameri and Simmet 2000; Lemak et al. 2007).Since DSE in interneurons has not been reported in amygdala and we demonstrated the existence of DSE in pyramidal cells of LA (Kodirov et al. 2010), we then studied the same phenomenon in regard to interneurons; the main question was whether or not does depolarization-induced mobilization of eCBs from the two types of postsynaptic LA neurons cause similar retrograde modulation of cortical inputs? Subsequently, DSE between the presynaptic terminals and interneurons was shown, and we found that its properties are similar to those in pyramidal cells of LA. Our study documents the participation of endogenous cannabinoids of interneurons in DSE.  相似文献   

16.
17.
Dopamine plays a critical role in behavioral tasks requiring interval timing (time perception in a seconds-to-minutes range). Although some studies demonstrate the role of dopamine receptors as a controller of the speed of the internal clock, other studies demonstrate their role as a controller of motivation. Both D1 dopamine receptors (D1DRs) and D2 dopamine receptors (D2DRs) within the dorsal striatum may play a role in interval timing because the dorsal striatum contains rich D1DRs and D2DRs. However, relative to D2DRs, the precise role of D1DRs within the dorsal striatum in interval timing is unclear. To address this issue, rats were trained on the peak-interval 20-sec procedure, and D1DR antagonist SCH23390 was infused into the bilateral dorsocentral striatum before behavioral sessions. Our results showed that the D1DR blockade drastically reduced the maximum response rate and increased the time to start responses with no effects on the time to terminate responses. These findings suggest that the D1DRs within the dorsal striatum are required for motivation to respond, but not for modulation of the internal clock speed.

Animals, including humans, can regulate their behavior according to temporal information. This suggests animals can perceive the passage of time (i.e., time perception). Without accurate time perception, we cannot speak, appreciate and play music, drive cars, and play sports. The current paper focussed on time perception in a seconds-to-minutes range; i.e., interval timing (Buhusi and Meck 2005). Interval timing is essential for an optimal foraging (Kacelnik and Bateson 1996) and associative learning (Gallistel and Gibbon 2000).In rodents, interval timing is frequently examined using the peak-interval (PI) procedure (Catania 1970; Roberts 1981). This task consists of randomly ordered two types of trials; i.e., fixed-interval (FI) and probe trials, separated by intertrial intervals (ITIs). During each FI trial, a reward is delivered for the first response made after a criterion time (e.g., 20 sec) has elapsed from the start of the trial, but not for responses made before the criterion time. Probe trials usually last for three times more than the criterion time in FI trials (e.g., 60 sec), and the reward is omitted. During an individual probe trial, rats typically start responding before the criterion time and stop it after the criterion time. The average rate of responses throughout the multiple probe trials in a session increases as the criterion time approaches, reaches a peak around the criterion time, and then decreases. The location of the peak of the response rate function (peak time), the spread of the function (peak spread), and the height of the peak (peak rate) are indices for timing accuracy, timing precision, and motivation to respond, respectively.Many studies have demonstrated that systemic injections of drugs affecting dopamine (DA) receptors impact interval timing in various ways. Some studies suggest that DA modulates the speed of the internal clock (Meck 1996). Systemic injection of DA receptor agonist decreases peak times, while that of DA receptor antagonist increases peak times. Importantly, the degree to which responding is altered is proportional to the duration being timed (Maricq et al. 1981; Maricq and Church 1983; Meck 1983, 1996; Matell et al. 2006). Therefore, it is necessary to use multiple target durations of a PI procedure (e.g., 20 and 40 sec) while examining the occurrence of any changes in clock speed mechanisms adequately. A recent study showed that transient activation or inhibition of DA neurons in the substantia nigra pars compacta (SNc) was sufficient to induce overestimation or underestimation of time in a temporal discrimination task, respectively (Soares et al. 2016). These results suggest that DA neurons in the SNc and DA receptors modulate the speed of the internal clock (“the dopamine-clock hypothesis”). Other studies, however, suggest that DA is involved in motivation during the interval timing task (Balcı 2014). Systemic injection of dopamine D1 receptors (D1DRs) antagonist SCH23390 reduced the peak rate (Drew et al. 2003), whereas systemic injection of DA agonist D-amphetamine increased the peak rate but reduced peak time and start time of responding without affecting stop time of responding (Taylor et al. 2007). The results of these studies are similar to those of studies that investigated the effect of manipulation of motivation on interval timing (Balcı 2014). Manipulations that decrease motivation (e.g., decreasing reward magnitude, prefeeding, and reward devaluation) reduced the peak rate, and sometimes delayed the start time (Roberts 1981; Ludvig et al. 2007; Galtress and Kirkpatrick 2009; Delamater et al. 2014, 2018). In contrast, opposite manipulations on motivation (e.g., increasing reward magnitude) caused earlier initiation of responding and a higher peak rate (Galtress and Kirkpatrick 2009). These findings support the idea that DA contributes to modulating motivation (Ikemoto et al. 2015). Therefore, DA might modulate motivation also during the timing task (“the dopamine-motivation hypothesis”).DA receptors within the dorsal striatum (dSTR) can be responsible for the effect of systemically injected DA agents on interval timing. Lesioning of the dSTR and damaged DA neurons in the SNc projecting to the dSTR caused the severe impairment of interval timing (Meck 2006b; Gouvêa et al. 2015; Mello et al. 2015), and optogenetic manipulation of DA neurons in the SNc affected the performance of the temporal discrimination task (Soares et al. 2016). These results strengthen the notion that the DA pathway from the SNc to the dSTR and the modulation of DA receptors in the dSTR are important for interval timing. Therefore, systemically injected DA agents might affect interval timing task through the DA receptors in the dSTR. Consistently, the affinity of DA agents for D2 dopamine receptors (D2DRs), but not D1DRs, correlated with the degree to produce the rightward shift of the temporal bisection function (Meck 1986), and systemic injection of D2DR blocker, but not D1DR blocker, affected interval timing in the PI procedure (Drew et al. 2003). These findings suggest that the change in the activity of D2DRs is implicated in the effect of DA on interval timing.Some studies, however, suggest that also the D1DRs within the dSTR might also play an important role in interval timing. A study showed that stimulation of the D1DR by systemic injection changed interval timing behavior (Cheung et al. 2007). D1DRs, as well as D2DRs, are highly expressed in the medium spiny neurons in the dSTR (Gerfen and Surmeier 2011). Thus, it is the possible that D1DRs in the dSTR also play an important role in interval timing. Notably, a recent study reported that D1DR blockade in the dSTR increased the time of stop responding (De Corte et al. 2019), and the findings cannot be explained by either “dopamine-clock hypothesis” nor “dopamine-motivation hypothesis.” Therefore, further studies are needed to clarify the role of D1DRs within the dSTR in interval timing.The present study aimed to examine further the role of D1DRs within the dorsal striatum in interval timing using the PI procedure. After a limited amount of training (Cheng et al. 2007), rats were infused with D1DR antagonist SCH23390 into the bilateral dSTR. Our findings support the “dopamine-motivation hypothesis” of D1DRs in the dSTR.  相似文献   

18.
Discrimination of sensory signals is essential for an organism to form and retrieve memories of relevance in a given behavioral context. Sensory representations are modified dynamically by changes in behavioral state, facilitating context-dependent selection of behavior, through signals carried by noradrenergic input in mammals, or octopamine (OA) in insects. To understand the circuit mechanisms of this signaling, we characterized the function of two OA neurons, sVUM1 neurons, that originate in the subesophageal zone (SEZ) and target the input region of the memory center, the mushroom body (MB) calyx, in larval Drosophila. We found that sVUM1 neurons target multiple neurons, including olfactory projection neurons (PNs), the inhibitory neuron APL, and a pair of extrinsic output neurons, but relatively few mushroom body intrinsic neurons, Kenyon cells. PN terminals carried the OA receptor Oamb, a Drosophila α1-adrenergic receptor ortholog. Using an odor discrimination learning paradigm, we showed that optogenetic activation of OA neurons compromised discrimination of similar odors but not learning ability. Our results suggest that sVUM1 neurons modify odor representations via multiple extrinsic inputs at the sensory input area to the MB olfactory learning circuit.

Behavioral choices depend on discrimination among “sensory objects,” which are neural representations of multiple coincident sensory inputs, across a range of sensory modalities. For example, “odor objects” (Gottfried 2009; Wilson and Sullivan 2011; Gire et al. 2013) are represented in sparse ensembles of neurons, that are coincidence detectors of multiple parallel inputs from odor quality channels. This principle is used widely in animals, including in mushroom bodies (MBs), the insect center for associative memory (Masuda-Nakagawa et al. 2005; Honegger et al. 2011), and in the piriform cortex (PCx) of mammals (Stettler and Axel 2009; Davison and Ehlers 2011).The selectivity of sensory representations can be modulated dynamically by changes in behavioral state, allowing an animal to learn and respond according to perceptual task. In mammals, the noradrenergic system originating in the locus coeruleus (LC) is implicated in signaling behavioral states such as attention, arousal and expectation (Aston-Jones and Cohen 2005; Sara and Bouret 2012).In insects, octopamine (OA), structurally and functionally similar to noradrenalin (NA) in mammals (Roeder 2005), can mediate changes in behavioral state that often promote activity; for example, sensitization of reflex actions in locusts (Sombati and Hoyle 1984), aggressive state in crickets (Stevenson et al. 2005), initiation and maintenance of flight state (Brembs et al. 2007; Suver et al. 2012), and enhanced excitability of Drosophila motion detection neurons during flight (Strother et al. 2018). Another role of OA is as a reward signal: A single OA neuron, VUMmx1, mediates the reinforcing function of unconditioned stimulus in the honeybee proboscis extension reflex (Hammer 1993; Hammer and Menzel 1998; Menzel 2012). In Drosophila, acquisition of appetitive memory is impaired in TβH mutants, unable to synthesize OA (Schwaerzel et al. 2003), and activation of OA neurons can substitute reinforcing stimulus in appetitive learning (Schroll et al. 2006). Moreover, OA receptors are necessary for reward learning in Drosophila (Burke et al. 2012) and crickets (Matsumoto et al. 2015).To understand the neural mechanisms of OA in higher order sensory discrimination, we used the simple sensory “cortex” of larval Drosophila, the calyx, which is the sensory input region of the mushroom bodies (MBs), the insect memory center. Here, each MB neuron (Kenyon cell [KC]) typically arborizes in several glomeruli, most of which are organized around the terminus of an olfactory projection neuron (PN); KCs thus combinatorially integrate multiple sensory input channels (Masuda-Nakagawa et al. 2005) and are coincidence detectors of multiple inputs. The APL provides inhibitory feedback (Lin et al. 2014; Masuda-Nakagawa et al. 2014) and helps to maintain KC sparse responses and odor selectivity (Honegger et al. 2011), analogous to inhibition in the mammalian PCx (Poo and Isaacson 2009; Stettler and Axel 2009; Gire et al. 2013). Thus, odors are represented as a sparse ensemble of KCs that are highly odor selective, a property beneficial for memory (Olshausen and Field 2004).In addition, the larval MB calyx is innervated by two OA neurons, sVUMmd1 and sVUMmx1, ventral unpaired medial neurons with dendritic fields originating in the mandibular and maxillary neuromeres, respectively, of the SEZ in the third instar larva (Selcho et al. 2014). sVUMmd1 and sVUMmx1 are named as OANa-1 and OANa-2, respectively, in the EM connectomic analysis of a 6-h first instar larva (Eichler et al. 2017; Supplemental Fig. 3 of Saumweber et al. 2018). These sVUM1 neurons also innervate the first olfactory neuropile of the antennal lobe (AL). This pattern of innervation is conserved in other insects, for example, the dorsal unpaired median (DUM) neurons in locusts (for review, see Bräunig and Pflüger 2001), the VUMmx1 neuron in honeybees (Hammer 1993; Schröter et al. 2007), and OA-VUMa2 neurons in adult Drosophila (Busch et al. 2009). In adult Drosophila, OA-VUMa2 neurons also show a dense innervation of the lateral horn, implicated in innate behaviors (Busch et al. 2009). The widespread innervation of the insect olfactory neuropiles also resembles the widespread NA innervation of mammalian olfactory processing areas, such as the olfactory bulb, and piriform cortex, by LC neurons originating in the brainstem.We characterized the innervation pattern and synaptic targets of sVUM1 neurons in the calyx, with MB intrinsic and also extrinsic neurons, the localization of the OA receptor Oamb in the calyx circuit, and the impact of sVUM1 neuron activation on behavioral odor discrimination. For this we used an appetitive conditioning paradigm, and tested the ability of larvae to discriminate between similar odors, as opposed to dissimilar odors. Since the larval connectome is based on a single brain, at first instar stage before octopaminergic connections have become as extensive as at third instar, and to obtain a comprehensive understanding of the synaptic targets of sVUM1s in the third-instar larval calyx, we extended our analysis to previously unanalyzed connectivity of VUM1s, to APL and PNs. Further, we combined light microscopy of third-instar larvae with the connectome described by Eichler et al. (2017).We find that sVUM1 neurons in third-instar larvae contact all the major classes of calyx neuron to some degree, consistent with EM synaptic analysis of the 6-h larva (Eichler et al. 2017). A GFP fusion of the OA receptor Oamb is localized in the terminals of PNs, and activating a subset of five SEZ neurons, including sVUM1 neurons, can affect discrimination of similar odors, without affecting underlying olfactory learning and memory ability. We suggest a broad modulatory effect of sVUM1 neurons in the calyx, including a potential role in modulating PN input at the second synapse in the olfactory pathway.  相似文献   

19.
In conditioned odor aversion (COA), the association of a tasteless odorized solution (the conditioned stimulus [CS]) with an intraperitoneal injection of LiCl (the unconditioned stimulus [US[), which produces visceral malaise, results in its future avoidance. The strength of this associative memory is mainly dependent on two parameters, that is, the strength of the US and the interstimuli interval (ISI). In rats, COA has been observed only with ISIs of ≤15 min and LiCl (0.15 M) doses of 2.0% of bodyweight, when tested 48 h after acquisition (long-term memory [LTM]). However, we previously reported a robust aversion in rats trained with ISIs up to 60 min when tested 4 h after acquisition (short-term memory [STM]). Since memories get reactivated during retrieval, in the current study we hypothesized that testing for STM would reactivate this COA trace, strengthening its LTM. For this, we compared the LTM of rats trained with long ISIs or low doses of LiCl initially tested for STM with that of rats tested for LTM only. Interestingly, rats conditioned under parameters sufficient to produce STM, but not LTM, showed a reliable LTM when first tested for STM. These observations suggest that under suboptimal training conditions, such as long ISIs or low US intensities, a CS–US association is established but requires reactivation in the short-term in order to persist in the long-term.

The dynamic and malleable nature of memories is a well-studied phenomenon. Traditionally, for memory formation to occur, a set of processes collectively known as consolidation are thought to be needed in order to stabilize memories, making them susceptible to modification during this period (Dudai et al. 2015). More recently a slightly distinct theory, known as memory integration, was proposed according to which memories are rapidly formed during learning without the need for consolidation, but any relevant information around the event can be integrated modifying them (Gisquet-Verrier and Riccio 2019). Common to both theories though, is that memories alternate between an inactive and an active state and modifications can mostly occur during the active state, which lasts for some time after learning, or during its reactivation due to retrieval (Lee et al. 2017; Albo and Gräff 2018; Gisquet-Verrier and Riccio 2019). Thus, memory malleability is explained either because consolidation can be altered or because additional information can be integrated with the initial memory (Bailey et al. 1996; Dudai 2004; Wixted 2004; Alberini et al. 2006; Lee et al. 2008, 2017; McGaugh and Roozendaal 2009; Roesler and Schröder 2011; Dudai et al. 2015; Nader 2015; Crossley et al. 2019; Gisquet-Verrier and Riccio 2019).In conditioned odor aversion (COA), an odorized tasteless solution (conditioned stimulus, CS) whose ingestion is followed by gastrointestinal malaise (unconditioned stimulus, US) is rejected in future encounters (conditioned response, CR). In most COA studies, a robust aversion has been observed only when the interstimulus interval (ISI) is ∼5 min, and no significant aversion can be seen when the ISI is >15 min (Hankins et al. 1973; Palmerino et al. 1980; Ferry et al. 1995, 1996; Ferry and di Scala 1997; Ferry et al. 2006; Chapuis et al. 2007). This observation has been attributed to a short-lasting memory of the odor that becomes unavailable for its association with the US after ISI >15 min. However, in all these instances the CR was measured 48 h after conditioning (LTM test), leaving up the possibility that CS–US association was formed but somehow did not last till the long-term. In keeping with this possibility, we previously reported a significant aversion during a test performed 4 h after conditioning (i.e., STM test) in rats trained with ISIs up to 60 min, three times longer than previously described (Tovar-Díaz et al. 2011). The LTM, however, was not tested so no further insight was provided regarding its persistence due to STM reactivation.Thus, in the current paper we hypothesized that a STM test would reactivate the initial memory, allowing it to further consolidate/integrate the information and to persist in the LTM. To test this possibility, we trained independent groups of rats with reduced US intensities or prolonged ISIs in a standard two-bottle choice COA paradigm and tested them twice at 4 and 48 h after conditioning. Our findings suggest that COA takes place under milder US and longer ISIs than previously thought and reactivating this memory during the STM test promotes its persistence in the LTM test.  相似文献   

20.
An adaptive memory system should prioritize information surrounding a powerful learning event that may prove useful for predicting future meaningful events. The behavioral tagging hypothesis provides a mechanistic framework to interpret how weak experiences persist as durable memories through temporal association with a strong experience. Memories are composed of multiple elements, and different mnemonic aspects of the same experience may be uniquely affected by mechanisms that retroactively modulate a weakly encoded memory. Here, we investigated how emotional learning affects item and source memory for related events encoded close in time. Participants encoded trial-unique category exemplars before, during, and after Pavlovian fear conditioning. Selective retroactive enhancements in 24-h item memory were accompanied by a bias to misattribute items to the temporal context of fear conditioning. The strength of this source memory bias correlated with participants’ retroactive item memory enhancement, and source misattribution to the emotional context predicted whether items were remembered overall. In the framework of behavioral tagging: Memory attribution was biased to the temporal context of the stronger event that provided the putative source of memory stabilization for the weaker event. We additionally found that fear conditioning selectively and retroactively enhanced stimulus typicality ratings for related items, and that stimulus typicality also predicted overall item memory. Collectively, these results provide new evidence that items related to emotional learning are misattributed to the temporal context of the emotional event and judged to be more representative of their semantic category. Both processes may facilitate memory retrieval for related events encoded close in time.

Emotional experiences gain privileged access to the neurobehavioral mechanisms of long-term memory (LaBar and Cabeza 2006; Kensinger 2009; McGaugh 2015; Yonelinas and Ritchey 2015; Mather et al. 2016). Importantly, this emotional enhancement of memory can spread to seemingly mundane details encoded close in time to the emotional experience. Through this temporal association, affectively neutral information retroactively acquires the capacity to predict emotional events, allowing us to better avoid or seek out those outcomes. However, what aspects of memory are modulated via temporal proximity to an emotional event? Episodic memories, for instance, are composed of stimulus information (e.g., item memory) embedded with contextual details (e.g., source memory) (Johnson et al. 1993; Tulving 2002). Emotion enhances item memory (Sharot and Phelps 2004; LaBar and Cabeza 2006; Mather 2007) but has inconsistent effects on contextual details associated with emotional stimuli (Yonelinas and Ritchey 2015). While there is evidence that episodic memory is selectively prioritized for related information encoded before (Dunsmoor et al. 2015) and after (Dunsmoor et al. 2015, 2018; Tambini et al. 2017; Keller and Dunsmoor 2020) an emotional experience, retroactive and proactive effects of emotional learning on source memory for this same information is unknown. Here, we investigated how temporal proximity to an emotional learning event influences both item memory and contextual details for related information.Enhancement in memory via a temporal association between mundane and salient events is consistent with neurobiological models of long-term memory. For example, the behavioral tagging hypothesis (derived from the synaptic tagging hypothesis; Frey and Morris 1997) proposes that weak learning is strengthened in memory if it is encoded within a critical time window of a more salient event and if the two events share overlapping neural ensembles (Moncada and Viola 2007; Ballarini et al. 2009; Wang et al. 2010; Takeuchi et al. 2016). As emotion is a powerful learning event, information encoded within temporal proximity may be strengthened in memory via a mechanism of behavioral tagging. The behavioral tagging hypothesis has been translated to humans using novelty (Fenker et al. 2008; Ballarini et al. 2013; Ramirez Butavand et al. 2020), threat (Dunsmoor et al. 2015), and reward (Patil et al. 2017) to induce memory enhancements for weakly encoded information encoded close in time. However, it is unclear whether and how this mechanism may affect memory for the contextual details associated with the weak event.Emotion can sometimes improve memory accuracy for information directly associated with an emotional stimulus, such as the spatial and temporal context in which the emotional stimulus was encoded (Kensinger and Schacter 2006; Schmidt et al. 2011; Rimmele et al. 2012; Talmi et al. 2019). Therefore, one possibility is that emotional learning improves both item and source memory for information in temporal proximity to an emotional event. In this case, we might expect item memory to be accompanied by source memory accuracy, such that items are appropriately organized in time relative to the emotional event. Alternatively, emotional learning might have no effect (Sharot and Yonelinas 2008; Wang and Fu 2010) or even impair memory for contextual information. Indeed, it may be simpler to remember a trivial item by virtue of associating the item with the salient context (Takashima et al. 2016). It is therefore plausible that linking weak and strong events by temporal proximity might improve item memory at the cost of source memory. Consequently, we may expect a relationship between the strength of item memory for related events encoded close in time and the strength of a misallocation bias to source the item to the more salient temporal context. This might suggest that retroactive memory enhancement for weakly encoded items relies in part on misattribution to the more salient emotional context.In addition to our exploration of how emotional learning impacts item and source memory for proximal events, we tested a parallel hypothesis that emotional learning alters abstract stimulus properties that may indirectly facilitate memory retrieval for those items as well. Previous work demonstrates that emotional learning is sensitive to how well an item represents its broader category; that is, typicality (Dunsmoor and Murphy 2014, 2015; Dunsmoor et al. 2014; Struyf et al. 2018; Lei et al. 2019). Whether emotional learning has the power to alter an abstract stimulus property such as subjective typicality is unknown, but stimulus memorability may be an underappreciated factor contributing to item memory (Bainbridge 2019). For instance, the hippocampus plays a role in both episodic memory and concept representations (Quiroga 2012; Davis and Poldrack 2014; Mack et al. 2016). Moreover, typical category members used as conditioned stimuli in Pavlovian fear conditioning preferentially engage the hippocampus and hippocampal-amygdala functional connectivity (Dunsmoor et al. 2014), which is a substrate for emotional memory enhancement (Murty et al. 2010).In the present study participants underwent a 2-d Pavlovian fear conditioning task that included trial-unique (i.e., nonrepeating) pictures of animals and tools as conditioned stimuli (CSs), based on the protocol from Dunsmoor et al. (2015). Items were encoded before, during, and after Pavlovian fear conditioning. We predicted that emotional learning would have divergent effects on 24-h episodic memory accuracy for CS items and the encoding temporal context. Specifically, we predicted that CSs semantically related to the fear conditioned category would be selectively remembered regardless of their temporal context (Dunsmoor et al. 2015), but that participants would have a bias to attribute these related CSs to the temporal context of fear conditioning. We also investigated whether emotional learning enhances subjective typicality for CS category members related to the fear conditioning category, and whether long-term memory would be influenced by how strongly an item was deemed to represent its superordinate category. Such findings might indicate that weak memories formed in temporal proximity to an emotional experience our bound to the more salient temporal context, and that emotional learning can alter abstract stimulus properties of weakly encoded information to make this information more memorable.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号