共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
2.
It has been argued that visual search is a valid model for human foraging. However, the two tasks differ greatly in terms
of the coding of space and the effort required to search. Here we describe a direct comparison between visually guided searches
(as studied in visual search tasks) and foraging that is not based upon a visually distinct target, within the same context.
The experiment was conducted in a novel apparatus, where search locations were indicated by an array of lights embedded in
the floor. In visually guided conditions participants searched for a target defined by the presence of a feature (red target
amongst green distractors) or the absence of a feature (green target amongst red and green distractors). Despite the expanded
search scale and the different response requirements, these conditions followed the pattern found in conventional visual search
paradigms: feature-present search latencies were not linearly related to display size, whereas feature-absent searches were
longer as the number of distractors increased. In a non-visually guided foraging condition, participants searched for a target
that was only visible once the switch was activated. This resulted in far longer latencies that rose markedly with display
size. Compared to eye-movements in previous visual search studies, there were few revisit errors to previously inspected locations
in this condition. This demonstrates the important distinction between visually guided and non-visually guided foraging processes,
and shows that the visual search paradigm is an equivocal model for general search in any context. We suggest a comprehensive
model of human spatial search behaviour needs to include search at a small and large scale as well as visually guided and
non-visually guided search. 相似文献
3.
The view that certain facial expressions of emotion are universally agreed on has been challenged by studies showing that the forced-choice paradigm may have artificially forced agreement. This article addressed this methodological criticism by offering participants the opportunity to select a none of these terms are correct option from a list of emotion labels in a modified forced-choice paradigm. The results show that agreement on the emotion label for particular facial expressions is still greater than chance, that artifactual agreement on incorrect emotion labels is obviated, that participants select the none option when asked to judge a novel expression, and that adding 4 more emotion labels does not change the pattern of agreement reported in universality studies. Although the original forced-choice format may have been prone to artifactual agreement, the modified forced-choice format appears to remedy that problem. 相似文献
4.
5.
6.
Previous experiments in this laboratory employing a search paradigm have found highly significant differences in the detectability of a briefly exposed target pattern as a function of the spatial location of the target when it is presented simultaneously with a number of discriminably different nontarget patterns. These detectability differences, at loci equidistant from the fovea, could not be accounted for by any known variation in retinal spatial resolution or by differential lateral masking effects of the target by nearby nontarget patterns. These observations led to the hypothesis that the target in these experiments was detected by a serial mechanism which "scanned" a persisting but rapidly degrading neural representation of the visual scene with increasing detection failures the later in time the scan processed the location occupied by the target. If this hypothesis is correct, then target detectability should vary inversely with the number of stimuli which must be examined. The present experiment confirmed this expectation. A mathematical model of such a serial scanning process also predicts other, less obvious, effects on target detectability which were observed when the number of nontarget patterns was changed. 相似文献
7.
We examined how the performance of a visual search task while studying a list of to-be-remembered words affects subsequent memory for those words by humans. Previous research had suggested that episodic context encoding is facilitated when the study phase of a memory experiment requires, or otherwise encourages, a visual search for the to-be-remembered stimuli, and theta-band oscillations are more robust when animals are searching their environment. Moreover, hippocampal theta oscillations are positively correlated with learning in animals. We assumed that a visual search task performed during the encoding of words for a subsequent memory test would induce an exploratory state that would mimic the one that is induced in animals when performing exploratory activities in their environment, and that the encoding of episodic traces would be improved as a result. The results of several experiments indicated that the performance of the search task improved free recall, but the results did not extend to yes–no or forced choice recognition memory testing. We propose that visual search tasks enhance the encoding of episodic context information but do not enhance the encoding of to-be-remembered words. 相似文献
8.
Dr. Harville Hendrix Ph.D. 《Pastoral Psychology》1977,25(3):157-172
Contemporary pastoral counseling includes three paradigmatic elements: the image of the counselor as priest; the location of the human problem at the organismic-environmental interface; and the diagnosis of the human problem as alienation from self and others. These assumptions ought to be supplemented by the image of the counselor as prophet, by location of the human problemalso at the interface of the self and the ultimate, and by a diagnosis of the human problem as alienation from transcendence. To be adequate as a response to the total human situation, pastoral counseling theory must be expanded to include consideration of the transcendent dimension of human existence, and pastoral counseling method and technique must be modified to overcome alienation from the ultimate by facilitating the actualization of transcendence.His article is a revision of the keynote address he presented to the American Association of Pastoral Counselors at their annual meeting in May 1976. 相似文献
9.
Kwang-Kuo Hwang 《Asian Journal of Social Psychology》2003,6(3):287-291
It is my privilege to be invited to write a response to K.S. Yang's reply to my critique of the methodology he used in his empirical research on individual modernity in Taiwan (Hwang, 2003). Yang's reply consists of three parts: The first part provides a quick overview of the methodology used in his individual traditionality and modernity (T/M) research in Taiwan. The second part presents his reply to my methodological criticisms. The third part is a plea for more and better individual T/M research in Asia. My response consists of three corresponding sections, although I will concentrate on the second part of Yang's text. 相似文献
10.
Pearce JM George DN 《Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes》2003,29(2):118-129
Pigeons received an odd-item search task that involved an array of 12 patterns containing 11 similar distractors and a single target. Pecks to the target resulted in the delivery of food. Accuracy was greater on trials when a distinctive feature was located in the target but not in the distractors, rather than when the feature was in the distractors but not in the target. This search asymmetry was influenced by the similarity of the target to the distractors. The results are similar to those obtained with humans. 相似文献
11.
Subjects searched for a digit which was not presented in an array of nine different digits. The identification rate was estimated from the percent correct. Well-trained subjects identified 6.5 digits with a 50-msec. exposure. This fast identification rate was expected from our procedure using a single answer, an inconsistent target, and equal probability that every digit would be a target. Naive subjects markedly improved their performance in the first two weeks of the trials which were given for a total of 24 days. 相似文献
12.
Visual search has memory 总被引:9,自引:0,他引:9
By monitoring subjects' eye movements during a visual search task, we examined the possibility that the mechanism responsible for guiding attention during visual search has no memory for which locations have already been examined. Subjects did reexamine some items during their search, but the pattern of revisitations did not fit the predictions of the memoryless search model. In addition, a large proportion of the refixations were directed at the target, suggesting that the revisitations were due to subjects' remembering which items had not been adequately identified. We also examined the patterns of fixations and compared them with the predictions of a memoryless search model. Subjects' fixation patterns showed an increasing hazard function, whereas the memoryless model predicts a flat function. Lastly, we found no evidence suggesting that fixations were guided by amnesic covert scans that scouted the environment for new items during fixations. Results do not support the claims of the memoryless search model, and instead suggest that visual search does have memory. 相似文献
13.
14.
Visual and acoustic confusability between a target item and background items was varied in a visual search task. Visual confusability was a highly significant source of difficulty while acoustic confusability had no effect. The results do not seem to be interpretable within a theory which assumes compulsory auditory encoding of visual information. 相似文献
15.
Visual and acoustic confusability between a target item and background items was varied in a visual search task. Visual confusability was a highly significant source of difficulty while acoustic confusability had no effect. The results do not seem to be interpretable within a theory which assumes compulsory auditory encoding of visual information. 相似文献
16.
John Holmgren 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1968,4(4):247-252
A task involving both the search for and recognition of one of two possible critical elements embedded in a set of noise elements was investigated with the aid of a mathematical model. The model consists of three processes, these being search, recognition, and decision. The first of two experiments attempted to show the operation of two separate bias parameters in the decision process. While the results were in the right direction, the data did not unequivocally establish the necessity of two bias parameters. In the second study, it was found that while a S’s ability to find a critical element in a display decreased as the number of noise elements in the display increased, his ability to recognize which critical element it was after finding it remained constant. This result was interpreted as supporting a strictly all-or-none view of the search process. 相似文献
17.
G. C. Grindley Valerie Townsend 《Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006)》1970,22(1):62-67
Two experiments have been made on the problem of visual search using six patterns which were geometrically simple, and familiar to the subject.
In each case the subject's task was to say where, among the complex of patterns, a particular pattern (the “test object”) appeared when the exposure of the whole display was so brief as to prevent scanning by the eyes. He could be informed visually (in Experiment I) or verbally (in Experiment II) which pattern was to be regarded as the “test object.”
In both experiments it was found that foreknowledge of what was to be the test object gave a significantly higher standard of accuracy than knowledge given later. This suggests that something analogous to visual search can occur without eye movements. 相似文献
In each case the subject's task was to say where, among the complex of patterns, a particular pattern (the “test object”) appeared when the exposure of the whole display was so brief as to prevent scanning by the eyes. He could be informed visually (in Experiment I) or verbally (in Experiment II) which pattern was to be regarded as the “test object.”
In both experiments it was found that foreknowledge of what was to be the test object gave a significantly higher standard of accuracy than knowledge given later. This suggests that something analogous to visual search can occur without eye movements. 相似文献
18.
19.
20.
Visual search and stimulus similarity 总被引:50,自引:0,他引:50