共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
William N. Dember Stephanie H. Martin Mary K. Hummer Steven R. Howe Richard S. Melton 《Current Psychology》1989,8(2):102-119
Procedures employed in the development and validation of a new Optimism & Pessimism Scale are described, and information about
test-retest reliability is reported. Internal consistency analyses and other data suggest that optimism and pessimism are
not polar opposites and bring up questions about the intrerelatedness of the cognitive and affective realms. The magnitude
of the correlation between pessimism and measures of anxiety suggests a link between this measure and the Negative Affectivity
construct, and the potential relationships among optimism, pessimism, the Pollyanna Principle, and Positive and Negative Affect
are discussed. Conceptually, it appears that optimism and pessimism may aid in psychological defense by helping to bind anxiety.
This report was based on theses submitted separately by the second and third authors to the Division of Graduate Studies and
Research of the University of Cincinnati, both in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the Master of Arts degree.
For reprints of the article or copies of the Optimism & Pessimism Scale, please direct requests to William N. Dember, Department
of Psychology, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, Ohio 45221. 相似文献
2.
William N. Dember Stephanie H. Martin Mary K. Hummer Steven R. Howe Richard S. Melton 《Current psychology (New Brunswick, N.J.)》1989,8(2):102-119
Procedures employed in the development and validation of a new Optimism & Pessimism Scale are described, and information about
test-retest reliability is reported. Internal consistency analyses and other data suggest that optimism and pessimism are
not polar opposites and bring up questions about the intrerelatedness of the cognitive and affective realms. The magnitude
of the correlation between pessimism and measures of anxiety suggests a link between this measure and the Negative Affectivity
construct, and the potential relationships among optimism, pessimism, the Pollyanna Principle, and Positive and Negative Affect
are discussed. Conceptually, it appears that optimism and pessimism may aid in psychological defense by helping to bind anxiety.
This report was based on theses submitted separately by the second and third authors to the Division of Graduate Studies and
Research of the University of Cincinnati, both in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the Master of Arts degree. 相似文献
3.
Kevin L. Burke A. Barry Joyner Daniel R. Czech Matthew J. Wilson 《Current Psychology》2000,19(2):129-136
The purpose of this investigation was to investigate the concurrent validity of the Life Orientation Test-Revised (LOT-R)
(Scheier et al., 1994) and Optimism/Pessimism Scale (OPS) (Dember et al., 1989) by administering each questionnaire to the
same group of participants. The 154 participants (84 men and 70 women) were volunteer university students enrolled in physical
activity classes at a southeastern university. The questionnaires were counterbalanced to reduce possible bias due to taking
one questionnaire before taking the other. Due to recent suggestions concerning scoring mechanisms for the LOT-R (Dember et
al., 1989; Hummer, Dember, Melton & Schefft, 1992; Lightsey, 1996; Marshall and Lang, 1990) the instrument was scored to produce
a unidimensional score and two subscale scores. The analysis suggested that the LOTR and OPS may not be measuring similar
constructs. The results revealed that the LOT-R may be measuring “trait” optimism and pessimism, while the OPS may be measuring
“state” optimism and pessimism. It was also suggested that future research investigate racial differences on these constructs. 相似文献
4.
Kevin L. Burke Ph.D. A. Barry Joyner Daniel R. Czech Matthew J. Wilson 《Current psychology (New Brunswick, N.J.)》2000,19(2):129-136
The purpose of this investigation was to investigate the concurrent validity of the Life Orientation Test-Revised (LOT-R)
(Scheier et al., 1994) and Optimism/Pessimism Scale (OPS) (Dember et al., 1989) by administering each questionnaire to the
same group of participants. The 154 participants (84 men and 70 women) were volunteer university students enrolled in physical
activity classes at a southeastern university. The questionnaires were counterbalanced to reduce possible bias due to taking
one questionnaire before taking the other. Due to recent suggestions concerning scoring mechanisms for the LOT-R (Dember et
al., 1989; Hummer, Dember, Melton & Schefft, 1992; Lightsey, 1996; Marshall and Lang, 1990) the instrument was scored to produce
a unidimensional score and two subscale scores. The analysis suggested that the LOTR and OPS may not be measuring similar
constructs. The results revealed that the LOT-R may be measuring “trait” optimism and pessimism, while the OPS may be measuring
“state” optimism and pessimism. It was also suggested that future research investigate racial differences on these constructs. 相似文献
5.
Mary K. Hummer William N. Dember Richard S. Melton Bruce K. Schefft 《Current psychology (New Brunswick, N.J.)》1992,11(1):37-50
This study tested the hypothesis that the moderate correlation between optimism (O) and pessimism (P) scores (their non-bipolarity)
found in earlier studies may be an artifact of the differential sensitivity of O and P items to two response sets: defensive
pessimism and Pollyannaism. The data failed to support either component of the hypothesis. Thus, defensive pessimists, as
measured in two quite different ways, had lower depression scores (BDI) than genuine pessimists, but contrary to prediction,
had higher depression scores than optimists. Moreover, deleting P items with a strong defensive quality from the P scale failed
to increase the correlation between O and P scores. Similarly, controlling Pollyannaism by a partial correlation procedure
failed to increase the strength of the relation between O and P. The results in general support the notion that optimism and
pessimism are not polar opposites.
This article is based on a dissertation presented by the senior author to the Division of Graduate Studies and Research of
the University of Cincinnati in partial fulfillment of the Doctor of Philosophy degree. 相似文献
6.
Mary K. Hummer William N. Dember Richard S. Melton Bruce K. Schefft 《Current Psychology》1992,11(1):37-50
This study tested the hypothesis that the moderate correlation between optimism (O) and pessimism (P) scores (their non-bipolarity)
found in earlier studies may be an artifact of the differential sensitivity of O and P items to two response sets: defensive
pessimism and Pollyannaism. The data failed to support either component of the hypothesis. Thus, defensive pessimists, as
measured in two quite different ways, had lower depression scores (BDI) than genuine pessimists, but contrary to prediction,
had higher depression scores than optimists. Moreover, deleting P items with a strong defensive quality from the P scale failed
to increase the correlation between O and P scores. Similarly, controlling Pollyannaism by a partial correlation procedure
failed to increase the strength of the relation between O and P. The results in general support the notion that optimism and
pessimism are not polar opposites.
This article is based on a dissertation presented by the senior author to the Division of Graduate Studies and Research of
the University of Cincinnati in partial fulfillment of the Doctor of Philosophy degree. 相似文献
7.
Three studies examined the relation between dispositional optimism and gambling. In Study 1, optimists were more likely than pessimists to have positive gambling expectations and report maintaining these expectations following losses. They also were more likely to indicate that winning money was a primary motivation for their gambling. Study 2 demonstrated that pessimists but not optimists reduce their betting and expectations after poor gaming performance. Study 3 replicated this effect using a more controlled experiment and showed that after losing, optimists report remembering more near wins than do pessimists. Thus, all three studies suggest that optimists, more than pessimists, maintain positive expectations and continue gambling after experiencing negative gaming outcomes. The authors suggest that despite optimism's many benefits, there are common situations in which the pessimistic tendency to disengage is beneficial. 相似文献
8.
Two studies examined motivational influences on and correlates of defensive pessimism and self-handicapping and investigated the relationship between these two cognitive strategies and performance attainment. The findings indicated that defensive pessimism and self-handicapping have similar motivational profiles, with the primary difference being that self-handicapping represents the absence of approach motivation in the achievement domain, as well as the presence of avoidance motivation. Self-handicapping, but not defensive pessimism, was shown to undermine performance-attainment, and performance-avoidance goals were validated as mediators of this negative relationship. Issues regarding the functional nature of the two cognitive strategies are discussed. 相似文献
9.
Research suggests that optimism feels good. However, does it always feel good? We suggest that the benefits (and costs) of optimism and pessimism depend on their timing. A study of exam score estimates revealed that, after controlling for actual exam performance, optimistic expectations are unrelated to how people feel immediately before feedback, in contrast to the common wisdom that optimism "feels good." Furthermore, optimism has costs after feedback-participants who predicted higher scores before feedback felt worse after learning their scores. Finally, people seem to be aware of the potential costs of optimism-participants who predicted higher scores before feedback also anticipated experiencing greater disappointment should they perform poorly. These findings suggest that people may proactively manage their expectations to avoid the costs of optimism. 相似文献
10.
People tend to overestimate their comparative likelihood of experiencing a rosy future. The present research suggests that one reason for this error is that when people compare their likelihood of experiencing an event with that of the average person, they focus on their own chances of experiencing the event and insufficiently consider the likelihood of the average person experiencing the event. As a consequence, people tend to think that they are more likely than the average person to experience common events and less likely than the average person to experience rare events. This causes unrealistic optimism in the case of common desirable events and rare undesirable events, but unrealistic pessimism in the case of rare desirable events and common undesirable events (Studies 1 and 2). Study 2 further suggests that both egocentrism and focalism underlie these biases. These results suggest that unrealistic optimism is not as ubiquitous as once thought. 相似文献
11.
The relationship between attributional style, depression and dreaming was explored by analysing dream reports from 80 subjects for evidence of attributional style using the Content Analysis of Verbatim Material (CAVE) technique. These scores were then compared with a waking measure of attributional style—the Expanded Attributional Style Questionnaire (EASQ) and with levels of depression as measured by the BDI. Contrary to expectations, dream attributional style did not correlate with waking attributional style, nor was there a significant correlation between internal, global and stable attributional style in dreams and level of depression. Results did support previous research that an internal, stable and global waking attributional style correlates with depression. 相似文献
12.
Optimism has been classically considered a key trait in entrepreneurs' personality but it has been studied from a psychological point of view only in recent years. The main aim of this research is to study the relationship between dispositional optimism, pessimism and realism as a function of the tendency to create technology-based businesses. A sample of undergraduate students (n= 205) filled in an electronic questionnaire containing the Life Orientation Test-Revised after they were classified as potential technological entrepreneurs, potential general entrepreneurs and non-potential entrepreneurs. Our results show that technology-based entrepreneurs are more optimistic than non-potential entrepreneurs, whereas there were no statistical differences in pessimism and realism. The results are interpreted theoretically to define the potential entrepreneur and, from an applied perspective, to design training programmes to support future technological entrepreneurs. 相似文献
13.
Recent studies confirm that dispositional pessimism is related with a worse state of physical health, in comparison with dispositional optimism . The efficacy of stress coping strategies has been considered as a mediating mechanism in this relationship. The aim of the present study is to analyze in a sample of university students (n = 200) the association between dispositional optimism/pessimism and different categories of self-reported somatic symptoms , and evaluate the possible mediation of the stress coping strategies in this relationship. The results indicate that: 1) pessimism is positively related with the report of physical symptoms, whereas optimism is negatively associated to somatic complaints; and 2) this relationship can be mainly explained by the differential utilization of the coping strategy denominated self-criticism (characteristic of pessimism). 相似文献
14.
Optimism and pessimism are personality variables that have repeatedly been shown to affect health, job performance, and social
relationships. Various instruments purport to measure these dispositions that differ substantially in their theoretical and
measurement models. While research has examined relations between subsets of these measures, their unique predictive ability
has been neglected. Three sequential studies evaluated the interrelation and predictive ability of the three most utilized
instruments, the Life Orientation Test (LOT), the O/P Instrument (OPI) and variants of the Attributional Style Questionnaire
(ASQ). Consistent with expectations, measures sharing a future-expectancy component (LOT and OPI) were moderately related
and most predictive of health, depression, and coping. ASQ measures were modestly related to the LOT and OPI and offered less
consistent predictions. 相似文献
15.
《British journal of psychology (London, England : 1953)》2006,97(1):115-135
This study investigated the impact of knowledge of results (KR) format on the performance and stress associated with a vigilance task. Also examined was the effect of the interaction of KR‐format (Hit‐KR, False Alarm‐KR, Miss‐KR, and a composite of all three formats) with dispositional optimism and pessimism on performance outcome and reported stress state. Hypotheses based upon a theory of feedback intervention were tested. KR regarding correct detections and the composite‐KR (KR regarding correct detections, false alarms, and missed signals) enhanced perceptual sensitivity. However, False Alarm‐KR and Miss‐KR did not. Contrary to expectations based on the theory, performance was unrelated to the traits across all KR conditions. However, the effects of KR‐format on self‐reports of stress depended on the individual's level of pessimism and optimism. In addition, KR format and personality affected the multiple dimensions of stress state in different ways. 相似文献
16.
Sean P. Reilley Andrew L. Geers Dawn L. Lindsay Laura Deronde William N. Dember 《Current Psychology》2005,24(1):43-59
Optimism and pessimism are personality variables that have repeatedly been shown to affect health, job performance, and social
relationships. Various instruments purport to measure these dispositions that differ substantially in their theoretical and
measurement models. While research has examined relations between subsets of these measures, their unique predictive ability
has been neglected. Three sequential studies evaluated the interrelation and predictive ability of the three most utilized
instruments, the Life Orientation Test (LOT), the O/P Instrument (OPI) and variants of the Attributional Style Questionnaire
(ASQ). Consistent with expectations, measures sharing a future-expectancy component (LOT and OPI) were moderately related
and most predictive of health, depression, and coping. ASQ measures were modestly related to the LOT and OPI and offered less
consistent predictions. 相似文献
17.
Attributional style in depression: a meta-analytic review 总被引:8,自引:0,他引:8
In this article we report meta-analyses of the relation of attributional styles to depression. In 104 studies involving nearly 15,000 subjects, several attributional patterns had reliable associations with depression scores. For negative events, attributions to internal, stable, and global causes had a reliable and significant association with depression. Studies in which the attribution factors of ability and luck were measured also showed a reliable association with depression. For positive events, attributions to external, unstable, and specific causes were associated with depression. Ability and luck attribution factors for positive events were also associated with depression. The relations for positive events, however, were weaker than the corresponding ones for negative events. In general, these patterns of relations were independent of a number of potential mediators suggested by authors in this literature, including the type of subject studied (psychiatric vs. college student), the type of event about which the attribution is made (real vs. simulated), the depression measure used, or the publication status of the research report. These conclusions are compared with those of other reviews. Implications for attributional models of depression are discussed. 相似文献
18.
19.
Prior research has established positive outcomes of health optimism (appraising one's health as good despite poor objective health (OH)) and negative outcomes of health pessimism (appraising health as poor despite good OH), yet little is known about their contributors. We examined the role of psychosocial factors (life event stress, depression, dispositional optimism, perceived social support) in health realism (appraising health in accordance with OH), optimism and pessimism among 489 older men and women. We then accounted for the psychosocial factors when examining multiple health correlates of health realism, optimism and pessimism. Controlling for age, gender and income, regression results indicate that depression and social support were associated with less health optimism, while dispositional optimism was associated with greater health optimism among those in poor OH. Dispositional optimism was associated with less health pessimism and life event stress was associated with greater pessimism among those in good OH. Beyond the effects of the psychosocial factors, structural equation model results indicate that health optimism was positively associated with healthy behaviours and perceived control over one's health; health pessimism was associated with poorer perceived health care management. Health optimism and pessimism have different psychosocial contributors and health correlates, validating the health congruence approach to later life well-being, health and survival. 相似文献
20.
Joel E. Milam Jean L. Richardson Gary Marks Carol A. Kemper Allen J. Mccutchan 《Psychology & health》2013,28(2):167-181
Relationships between dispositional optimism and pessimism and the course of HIV infection, determined by changes in viral load and CD4 counts, were studied in a longitudinal cohort of 412 patients on antiretroviral therapy (ART). Multiple regression analyses controlling for baseline levels of disease status, ethnicity, and depressive symptoms demonstrated that higher pessimism at baseline was associated with higher viral load at follow-up (average of 18 months later). Optimism at baseline had a curvilinear relationship with CD4 counts at follow-up. Moderate levels of optimism at baseline predicted the highest CD4 counts at follow-up. Although optimism and pessimism were associated with specific health behaviors (e.g., ART adherence, cigarette use, drug use, dietary practices), none of these behaviors mediated the optimism/pessimism effects. The biologic and behavioral mediators of associations of personality variables with the course of treated HIV infection deserve continued investigation. 相似文献