共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
A group of circuits is described that produce various patterns on an oscilloscope screen that are useful in studying vision. These patterns are: a flashing bar, a drifting bar, a flashing grating, and a drifting grating. The circuits have controls for: contrast, velocity, width, position, and orientation with respect to the vertical. This article describes the principles of design and use of these circuit modules as a precise electronic visual stimulator. 相似文献
2.
3.
An intracranial stimulation unit which offers precise control of stimulus duration, frequency, and current is described. The unit generates a square wave stimulus output utilizing very inexpensive, easily constructed integrated circuitry. 相似文献
4.
5.
THURLOW W 《The American journal of psychology》1957,70(4):653-654
6.
7.
8.
9.
An auditory cue-depreciation effect. 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
An experiment is reported in which subjects first heard a list of words and then tried to identify these same words from degraded utterances. Paralleling previous findings in the visual modality, the probability of identifying a given utterance was reduced when the utterance was immediately preceded by other, more degraded, utterances of the same word. A second experiment replicated this "cue-depreciation effect" and in addition found the effect to be weakened, if not eliminated, when the target word was not included in the initial list or when the test was delayed by two days. 相似文献
10.
11.
M E Walker 《Perception & psychophysics》1989,45(4):315-322
The present study, using the nonhierarchical overlapping clustering algorithm MAPCLUS to fit the Shepard-Arabie (1979) ADCLUS model, attempted to derive a set of features that would accurately describe the auditory alphabet confusions present in the data matrices of Conrad (1964) and Hull (1973). Separate nine-cluster solutions accounted for 80% and 89% of the variance in the matrices, respectively. The clusters revealed that the most frequently confused letter names contained common vowels and phonetically similar consonants. Further analyses using INDCLUS, an individual differences extension of the MAPCLUS algorithm and ADCLUS model, indicated that while the patterns of errors in the two matrices were remarkably similar, some differences were also apparent. These differences reflected the differing amounts of background noise present in the two studies. 相似文献
12.
Stephen R. Arnott Anthony Singhal Melvyn A. Goodale 《Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience》2009,9(3):335-342
Despite a widespread familiarity with the often compelling urge to yawn after perceiving someone else yawn, an understanding
of the neural mechanism underlying contagious yawning remains incomplete. In the present auditory fMRI study, listeners used
a 4-point scale to indicate how much they felt like yawning following the presentation of a yawn, breath, or scrambled yawn
sound. Not only were yawn sounds given significantly higher ratings, a trait positively correlated with each individual’s
empathy measure, but relative to control stimuli, random effects analyses revealed enhanced hemodynamic activity in the right
posterior inferior frontal gyrus (pIFG) in response to hearing yawns. Moreover, pIFG activity was greatest for yawn stimuli
associated with high as opposed to low yawn ratings and for control sounds associated with equally high yawn ratings. These
results support a relationship between contagious yawning and empathy and provide evidence for pIFG involvement in contagious
yawning. A supplemental figure for this study may be downloaded from http://cabn.psychonomic-journals.org/content/supplemental. 相似文献
13.
Two experiments were designed to investigate the occurrence of atemporal aftereffect following auditory spatial stimulation. The task required Ss to compare by means of a motorresponse the duration of a test tone presented at a variable interval after stimulation with a standard tone. In both experiments the posttest duration was underestimated relative to the pretest duration, i.e., there was a temporal aftereffect(TAE). A control experiment which involved Ss making estimates of the duration of the test tones, without the presentation of interpolated standard tones, did not show this effect. The temporal aftereffect followed a function analogous to the “distance paradox” forspatial aftereffects. 相似文献
14.
Previous research has found that pictures (e.g., a picture of an elephant) are remembered better than words (e.g., the word
"elephant"), an empirical finding called the picture superiority effect (Paivio & Csapo. Cognitive Psychology 5(2):176-206, 1973). However, very little research has investigated such memory differences for other types of sensory stimuli (e.g. sounds
or odors) and their verbal labels. Four experiments compared recall of environmental sounds (e.g., ringing) and spoken verbal labels of those sounds (e.g., "ringing"). In contrast to earlier studies that have shown no difference
in recall of sounds and spoken verbal labels (Philipchalk & Rowe. Journal of Experimental Psychology 91(2):341-343, 1971; Paivio, Philipchalk, & Rowe. Memory & Cognition 3(6):586-590, 1975), the experiments reported here yielded clear evidence for an auditory analog of the picture superiority effect. Experiments
1 and 2 showed that sounds were recalled better than the verbal labels of those sounds. Experiment 2 also showed that verbal
labels are recalled as well as sounds when participants imagine the sound that the word labels. Experiments 3 and 4 extended
these findings to incidental-processing task paradigms and showed that the advantage of sounds over words is enhanced when
participants are induced to label the sounds. 相似文献
15.
16.
Soto-Faraco S 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》2000,26(1):264-278
Previous studies of the auditory analogue of repetition blindness have led to different conclusions regarding the nature of the effect (e.g., N. Kanwisher & M. C. Potter, 1989; M. Miller & D. MacKay, 1994). In the present study, recall accuracy for repeated elements was examined with lists of 2 or 3 items presented dichotically under high temporal pressure. When this procedure was used, a repetition deficit in recall was obtained for both vowels (Experiment 1) and consonant-vowel syllables (Experiment 2). Further experiments demonstrated that this deficit decreases as the stimulus onset asynchrony between the 2 critical elements increases (Experiment 3) and showed that the effect also occurs for words and not just nonsense syllables (Experiment 4). In all 4 experiments, estimations of guessing biases showed that responses to unrepeated lists were not artificially favored over responses to repeated lists. 相似文献
17.
A microcomputer-based instrument is described that provides visual stimuli for a CRT display and collects and processes response data. The system produces a variety of visual patterns to facilitate the study of the properties of visual neurons of several different animals. The visual patterns include drifting sinusoidal or bar gratings, contrast reversal gratings, and diffuse light. By means of a keypad, the experimenter can select the visual pattern and its parameters, such as depth of modulation, spatial frequency, initial spatial phase, temporal frequency, and rotation angle. 相似文献
18.
A series of “auditory Stroop” experiments is described. These demonstrate an effect of stimulus words presented on speed of judgments of speaker gender and, conversely, an influence of speaker gender on judgments of words presented. In an experiment in which responses to speaker gender were semantically related to, but not identical with, stimulus words, the auditory Stroop effect was attenuated but remained in evidence. Potential parallels between this auditory paradigm and the visual Stroop color/word effect are explored, and it is suggested that the Stroop effects in the two modalities operate along broadly similar lines. The search for a common causal mechanism would therefore be justified. 相似文献
19.
20.
In many psychophysical experiments, the participant's task is to detect small changes along a given stimulus dimension or to identify the direction (e.g., upward vs. downward) of such changes. The results of these experiments are traditionally analyzed with a constant-variance Gaussian (CVG) model or a high-threshold (HT) model. Here, the authors demonstrate that for changes along three basic sound dimensions (frequency, intensity, and amplitude-modulation rate), such models cannot account for the observed relationship between detection thresholds and direction-identification thresholds. It is shown that two alternative models can account for this relationship. One of them is based on the idea of sensory quanta; the other assumes that small changes are detected on the basis of Poisson processes with low means. The predictions of these two models are then compared against receiver operating characteristics (ROCs) for the detection of changes in sound intensity. It is concluded that human listeners' perception of small and unidimensional acoustic changes is better described by a discrete-state Poisson model than by the more commonly used CVG model or by the less favored HT and quantum models. 相似文献