共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
2.
Gerrit Hirschfeld Heidrun Bien Meinou de Vries Heidi Lüttmann Johannes Schwall 《Behavior research methods》2010,42(2):542-546
Several software programs exist to assist researchers in setting up online questionnaires. Existing tools are of little help
for delivering online rating studies, for which it is often desirable to collect data from participants for only a subset
of a stimulus set. OR-Vis enables researchers to quickly set up online rating studies by supplying the set of items to be
rated, the number of stimuli an individual participant responds to, the number of participants an item is shown to, and the
rating questions. The software then generates and delivers unique questionnaires for each participant, while managing the
data collection process. The present article describes OR-Vis, its installation process, and how to use it to gather data.
OR-Vis is open-source software and can be downloaded from www.orvis.uni-muenster.de. 相似文献
3.
4.
Before switching a laboratory from analog to digital, for the recording of video files for use in Noldus software such as
Ethovision and The Observer, researchers need to proceed with caution. There are obvious advantages in moving to digital recording
for behavioral work, including increased storage capacity; no requirement to purchase video tapes; immediate search by date,
time, or event; digital images are of higher quality; ability to view study sites remotely by Internet connection; and “smart”
features, such as motion detection. But before you throw away your time-lapse video recorders, time code generators, and video
multiplexors, there are some important cautions to take account of. Some research groups have bought digital surveillance
systems on the assumption that they work with Ethovision and The Observer, only to be disappointed. The vast majority of systems
depend on proprietary compression software that must then be converted to work properly in Ethovision or The Observer. 相似文献
5.
Caroline Smith Barbara Lloyd Colin Crook 《Current psychology (New Brunswick, N.J.)》1982,2(1-3):289-292
The coding system described was originally devised to facilitate the analysis of mother-infant interaction. Our aims were to provide accurate measures of the duration of different activities, to enable identification of particular actions in order to carry out sequential analyses, and to allow systematic assessment and improvement of reliability. At the time recording began, there appeared to be no readily available systems which achieved all of these aims. The system described here enables coded videotape material to be examined with the assistance of a computer. This is implemented through two subsystems, those of record and playback. In the playback subsystem the behaviour units identified by a coder are mapped on to this record. This results in a protocol of behaviour on a highly accurate time base. 相似文献
6.
The coding system described was originally devised to facilitate the analysis of mother-infant interaction. Our aims were
to provide accurate measures of the duration of different activities, to enable identification of particular actions in order
to carry out sequential analyses, and to allow systematic assessment and improvement of reliability. At the time recording
began, there appeared to be no readily available systems which achieved all of these aims.
The system described here enables coded videotape material to be examined with the assistance of a computer. This is implemented
through two subsystems, those of record and playback. In the playback subsystem the behaviour units identified by a coder
are mapped on to this record. This results in a protocol of behaviour on a highly accurate time base. 相似文献
7.
We describe some programming tools for real-time monitoring and control of digital laboratory devices. These create several interrupt-driven processes that operate in the “background” of the user’s program. An event buffer logs switch closures, thus helping the user’s program keep up with events. Other simple commands (e.g., TurnOn) take a line number and a time argument as parameters; they are automatically executed at the time specified. Applications to operant conditioning are discussed. 相似文献
8.
9.
An Apple IIe-based system is described that generates both machine-readable and human-readable numbers for recording with visual information on videotape. Hardware and software components of the encoding system are detailed. An automatic decoding system that reads the numbers from videotape into computer memory is also described. Current applications of the system are briefly discussed. 相似文献
10.
Adrian Voßkühler Volkhard Nordmeier Lars Kuchinke Arthur M. Jacobs 《Behavior research methods》2008,40(4):1150-1162
In the present article, a new software is introduced that allows the recording and analyzing of eye- and mouse-tracking data
from slideshow-based experiments in parallel. The Open Gaze and Mouse Analyzer (OGAMA) is written in C#.NET and has been released
as an open-source project. Its main features include slide-show design, the recording of gaze and mouse data, database-driven
preprocessing and filtering of gaze and mouse data, the creation of attention maps, areas-of-interest definition, and replay.
Eyetracking and/or presentation soft- and hardware recordings in ASCII format can be imported. Data output is provided that
can be used directly with different statistical software packages. Because it is open source, one can easily adapt it to suit
one’s needs. 相似文献
11.
Lucas P. J. J. Noldus Rudie J. H. Trienes André H. M. Hendriksen Han Jansen Roland G. Jansen 《Behavior research methods》2000,32(1):197-206
The Observer Video-Pro is a system for collecting, managing, analyzing, and presenting observational data. It integrates The Observer software with time code and multimedia hardware components. It extends the functionality of a conventional real-time event recording program in various ways. Observational data can be collected, reviewed, and edited with synchronized display of the corresponding video images. For optimal visual feedback during coding, one can display the video image in a window on the computer screen. Video playback from either a VCR or a digital media file can be controlled by the computer, allowing software-controlled jog, shuttle, and search functions. Besides a wide range of VCRs, The Observer Video-Pro supports all major digital video file formats. The software allows the user to summarize research findings in numerical, graphical, or multimedia format. One can create a time-event plot for a quick glance at the temporal structure of the observed process, or run specific analysis procedures and generate reports with statistics. An Event Summary function is available for exploratory and qualitative analysis. Video material can be summarized in a Video Play List, which allows on-screen summary presentations or the creation of highlight compilations on tape, CD, or other media. Video images can be captured and saved as disk files, for use as illustrations in documents, slides for presentations, and so forth. In this paper we describe the design and operation of the system, illustrated with a case study from research on Repetitive Strain Injury (RSI). 相似文献
12.
Although much learning in real-life environments relies on highly selective feedback about outcomes, virtually all cognitive models of learning, judgment, and categorization assume complete and representative feedback. We investigated empirically the effect of selective feedback on decision making and how people code experience with selective feedback. The results showed that, in contrast to a commonly raised concern, performance was not impaired following learning with selective and biased feedback. Furthermore, even in a simple decision task, the experience that people acquired was not a mere recording of the observed outcomes, but rather a reconstruction from general task knowledge. 相似文献
13.
A technique is described that permits precise synchronization of video recorded behavior with discrete stimuli and responses. These discrete events are recorded as digital data on the video channel of a video recorder on a horizontal line that lies above the visible video information. These data may be observed in single-frame playback mode by underscanning the playback monitor. A circuit is described for computerized decoding of the digital data. Because each video field is uniquely coded, an updating of the data is possible 60 times each second. The described technique has several advantages over current approaches for synchronizing discrete stimulus and response events with video recorded behavior. 相似文献
14.
In this study, we used a paradigm similar to the one used by Milgram in his classic obedience study, using an immersive video environment. We manipulated the victim's degree of visibility and his ethnicity. When the victim was hidden, the level of obedience we obtained was similar to Milgram's. Replicating previous findings observed in real environments, participants were more obedient when the victim was hidden than when he was visible, and the more obedient participants negated their own responsibility by projecting responsibility on both the victim and the experimenter. State‐anger and right‐wing authoritarianism (RWA) emerged as two significant predictors of the level of obedience. Illustrating an underlying process of racial‐dehumanization, participants reported less anxiety and distress when the victim was a North African than when the victim was of the same racial origin as the participant. These results underscore the usefulness of using immersive environments when studying extreme social behaviors. Copyright © 2009 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 相似文献
15.
16.
17.
Waller D 《Memory & cognition》2006,34(3):491-504
Much contemporary research has suggested that memories for spatial layout are stored with a preferred orientation. The present research examined whether spatial memories are also stored with a preferred viewpoint position. Participants viewed images of an arrangement of objects taken from a single viewpoint and subsequently were tested on their ability to recognize the arrangement from novel viewpoints that had been translated in either the lateral or the depth dimension. Lateral and forward displacements of the viewpoint resulted in increasing response latencies and errors. Backward displacement showed no such effect, nor did lateral translation that resulted in a centered "canonical" view of the arrangement. These results further constrain the specificity of spatial memory, while also providing some evidence that nonegocentric spatial information is coded in memory. 相似文献
18.
Cassandra D. Gipson 《Learning and motivation》2008,39(4):285-295
Previous research with the radial maze has found evidence that rats can remember both places that they have already been (retrospective coding) and places they have yet to visit (prospective coding; Cook, R. G., Brown, M. F., & Riley, D. A. (1985). Flexible memory processing by rats: Use of prospective and retrospective information in the radial maze. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behaviour Processes, 11, 453-469). Such dual coding also has been found in pigeons using a radial maze analog in which insertion of a delay at different points during a trial affects performance differentially depending on where in the trial it is inserted. When a delay is interpolated either early or late in a trial, there is minimal disruption of performance compared with when it is interpolated in the middle of the trial. However, the analysis required with this procedure requires the assumption that if errors made on control trials are subtracted from errors made in delay trials, the remaining errors can be directly attributed to the delay. But errors may also be attributed to the changing criterion for making a response as the trial proceeds. Furthermore, the animal’s tendency to choose alternatives in a systematic order may also affect its need to remember the sequence of choices made (and yet to be made) on each trial. In the present research, we avoided having to make this assumption by giving the pigeons a two-alternative choice at the time of testing and by randomly determining for the pigeon the order of predelay choices on each trial. This change in procedure resulted in comparable performance as a function of where in the trial the test occurred on both control and delay trials. The effect of the delay was to produce a general decrement in performance independent of where it occurred in the trial. 相似文献
19.
The movements that we make with our body vary continuously along multiple dimensions. However, many of the tools and techniques presently used for coding and analyzing hand gestures and other body movements yield categorical outcome variables. Focusing on categorical variables as the primary quantitative outcomes may mislead researchers or distort conclusions. Moreover, categorical systems may fail to capture the richness present in movement. Variations in body movement may be informative in multiple dimensions. For example, a single hand gesture has a unique size, height of production, trajectory, speed, and handshape. Slight variations in any of these features may alter how both the speaker and the listener are affected by gesture. In this paper, we describe a new method for measuring and visualizing the physical trajectory of movement using video. This method is generally accessible, requiring only video data and freely available computer software. This method allows researchers to examine features of hand gestures, body movement, and other motion, including size, height, curvature, and speed. We offer a detailed account of how to implement this approach, and we also offer some guidelines for situations where this approach may be fruitful in revealing how the body expresses information. Finally, we provide data from a small study on how speakers alter their hand gestures in response to different characteristics of a stimulus to demonstrate the utility of analyzing continuous dimensions of motion. By creating shared methods, we hope to facilitate communication between researchers from varying methodological traditions. 相似文献
20.
Virginia Hatchette Arthur R. Zivian Marilyn T. Zivian Ronald Okada 《Behavior research methods》1999,31(1):19-23
STAZ is an interactive computer program that demonstrates statistical concepts, many of which cannot be readily demonstrated using conventional methods. Use of dynamic graphics encourages active engagement with challenging statistical concepts. The program consists of 13 graphical demonstrations, most of which allow for interactive participation by students. A detailed Help file with guided explanations accompanies each demonstration. STAZ is a multiple document interface program that makes full use of Windows features, such as tiling, links, and multitasking. Designed to be used as a supplement for any undergraduate statistics course, STAZ may be used by either instructors in classroom settings or students working independently. 相似文献