首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到19条相似文献,搜索用时 93 毫秒
1.
在治疗口腔颌面部脉管畸形的众多治疗方法中,激光治疗和瘤腔内药物注射是两种最为常用而且有效的非手术治疗手段.本文通过结合两者各自的优缺点,利用比较治疗学相关原理和方法,从治疗方法、治疗效果、治疗费用等方面对两者进行比较,以便获得一些启发,为临床治疗提供一定的借鉴.  相似文献   

2.
手术和血管内介入是目前临床上治疗颅内动脉瘤的有效方法,针对两者的优缺点,从治疗方式、疗效、费用等方面进行比较,为临床治疗思路提供借鉴。  相似文献   

3.
手术和血管内介入是目前临床上治疗颅内动脉瘤的有效方法,针对两者的优缺点,从治疗方式、疗效、费用等方面进行比较,为临床治疗思路提供借鉴.  相似文献   

4.
大肠癌是常见的恶性肿瘤,提高其总体疗效的有效途径是早期诊断和治疗。外科手术和内镜下治疗是目前临床上常用的治疗早期大肠癌的有效方法。针对两者的优缺点,本文利用比较治疗学相关原理和方法,从其适应证、疗效、并发症及费用等方面进行比较,以获得启示,为临床实践提供指导。  相似文献   

5.
大肠癌是常见的恶性肿瘤,提高其总体疗效的有效途径是早期诊断和治疗.外科手术和内镜下治疗是目前临床上常用的治疗早期大肠癌的有效方法.针对两者的优缺点,本文利用比较治疗学相关原理和方法,从其适应证、疗效、并发症及费用等方面进行比较,以获得启示,为临床实践提供指导.  相似文献   

6.
护理健康教育与治疗性沟通的比较与思考   总被引:6,自引:0,他引:6  
根据近年来有关护理健康教育和治疗性沟通的研究,从护理健康教育和治疗性沟通的概念、适用范畴、实施的模式、实施的程序、实施的主客体等角度对两者进行比较和思考,以解决目前临床对两者的困惑。提出治疗性沟通和规范护理健康教育协同进步才是护理学发展之未来。  相似文献   

7.
椎体成形术(PVP)与球囊扩张椎体后凸成形术(PKP)是目前临床上常用的治疗老年椎体骨质疏松性压缩骨折有效方法。针对两者的优缺点,利用比较治疗学相关原理和方法,从其生物力学、适应证、疗效、并发症及费用等方面进行比较,以获得启示,为临床实践提供指导。  相似文献   

8.
根据近年来有关护理健康教育和治疗性沟通的研究,从护理健康教育和治疗性沟通的概念、适用范畴、实施的模式、实施的程序、实施的主客体等角度对两者进行比较和思考,以解决目前临床对两者的困惑.提出治疗性沟通和规范护理健康教育协同进步才是护理学发展之未来.  相似文献   

9.
椎体成形术(PVP)与球囊扩张椎体后凸成形术(PKP)是目前临床上常用的治疗老年椎体骨质疏松性压缩骨折有效方法.针对两者的优缺点,利用比较治疗学相关原理和方法,从其生物力学、适应证、疗效、并发症及费用等方面进行比较.以获得启示,为临床实践提供指导.  相似文献   

10.
分析比较切开复位内固定和人工桡骨小头置换方法治疗桡骨小头粉碎性骨折(MasonⅢ型)的临床疗效。回顾分析2012年10月~2014年3月收治的24例桡骨小头粉碎性骨折(MasonⅢ型)的临床资料,其中切开复位内固定治疗17例;桡骨小头置换7例,平均随访12个月(6个月~18个月)。根据Broberg Morrey评分系统以及术后影像进行评价比较,同时比较两组住院天数以及费用。结果评价比较显示两者疗效均佳,术后两者活动度和功能评分相近,两组住院天数相近,内固定组费用更低。桡骨小头粉碎性骨折须尽早进行解剖复住功能重建,切开复位内固定和桡骨小头置换疗效相当。  相似文献   

11.
This paper argues that we can benefit or harm people by creating them, but only in the sense that we can create things that are good or bad for them. What we cannot do is to confer comparative benefits and harms to people by creating them or failing to create them. You are not better off (or worse off) created than you would have been had you not been created, for nothing has value for you if you do not exist, not even neutral value.  相似文献   

12.
《Philosophical Papers》2012,41(2):295-314
Abstract

Collectivities, just like individuals, exist, can act, bear responsibility for their acts and omissions, and be guilty. It sometimes makes sense to hold them responsible for what they do, or don't do, and to punish them for their misdeeds. With respect to many collectivities there is no practical purpose in holding them responsible, since there is not way that we can bring them to justice. But there are exceptions from this rule. In particular it is plausible to assume that sanctions against entire nations or peoples or populations living in open and democratic states may be an effective means to setting them straight where, collectively, they act wrongly. The best present example of this seems to be the Israelis.  相似文献   

13.
James WH  West C  Deters KE  Armijo E 《Adolescence》2000,35(139):455-465
Adolescents' responses to the Youth Dating Violence Survey have previously been documented by the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (1996). The present study on dating violence examined the responses of thirty-seven adolescents enrolled in an alternative high school program. Many reported psychological victimization in a dating relationship: their partners did something to make them feel jealous, damaged their possessions, said things to hurt their feelings, insulted them in front of others, tried to control them, threatened them, blamed them for bad things the dating partners did, and brought up something from the past to hurt them. In terms of perpetrating psychological abuse in a dating relationship, over half of the adolescents reported that they hurt their dating partners' feelings, insulted them in front of others, did something just to make them jealous, tried to control them, and damaged their possessions. Many of the adolescents had also been victims of physical violence in their dating relationships; they reported being scratched, slapped, slammed or held against a wall, kicked, bitten, forced to have sex, choked, and pushed, grabbed, or shoved, as well as having their arms twisted and fingers bent. Some perpetrated physical violence in dating situations, such as scratching their dating partners, hitting them with a fist or something hard, throwing something that hit their dating partners, kicking them, slapping them, physically twisting their arms, slamming or holding them against a wall, bending their fingers, biting them, choking them, and pushing, grabbing, or shoving them. The findings confirm that dating violence among adolescents is a serious health problem that needs to be addressed.  相似文献   

14.
In Experiment 1, the free-recall performance of young children, college students, and older adults was examined. Subjects encoded words by simply learning them, by studying them in either base or elaborate sentence frames, or by constructing sentences. Overall recall was better for the college students than for the children or for the older adults, and the college students recalled best in the simple learning condition. The young children recalled best in the sentence construction condition; recall by older adults did not vary as a function of the encoding tasks. In Experiment 2, college students and older adults recalled a categorized list, encoding the words by simply learning them, by studying them in elaborate sentence frames, or by completing word fragments. For both age groups, simple learning produced the highest level of recall. These results suggest that organization provides the most effective encoding system and that older adults may need a more obvious basis for organization than do younger adults. Younger and older adults recalled equally well only when organization was discouraged by conceptual processing.  相似文献   

15.
16.
Abstract— Intense hedonic states trigger psychological processes that are designed to attenuate them, and thus intense states may abate more quickly than mild states. Because people are unaware of these psychological processes, they may mistakenly expect intense states to last longer than mild ones. In Study 1, participants predicted that the more they initially disliked a transgressor, the longer their dislike would last. In Study 2, participants predicted that their dislike for a transgressor who hurt them a lot would last longer than their dislike for a transgressor who hurt them a little, but precisely the opposite was the case. In Study 3, participants predicted that their dislike for a transgressor who hurt them a lot would last longer than their dislike for a transgressor who hurt someone else a lot, but precisely the opposite was the case. These errors of prediction are discussed as instances of a more general phenomenon known as the region-β paradox.  相似文献   

17.
通过两项研究考查大学生在社会困境中的行为决策及公平感体验,检验社会价值取向的影响作用,有效被试分别为85名和84名。结果表明:(1)亲社会者比亲自我者更倾向做公平决策;信息对称比不对称条件更促进被试做公平决策;女生比男生更倾向做公平决策。(2)性别在社会价值取向与信息对称性对公平决策的影响中起调节作用,社会价值取向显著影响男生的公平决策,信息对称性显著影响女生的公平决策。(3)社会价值取向显著影响个体对不公平的容忍度,亲社会者更不容忍不公平行为。(4)对于利己的不公平行为,被试都倾向接受,亲社会者并不比亲自我者体验到更强的内疚情绪;对于不利己的不公平行为,被试都倾向拒绝,且产生较强的不公平感及气愤情绪。  相似文献   

18.
It is almost a truism that disagreement produces conflict. This article suggests that perceptions of bias can drive this relationship. First, these studies show that people perceive those who disagree with them as biased. Second, they show that the conflict-escalating approaches that people take toward those who disagree with them are mediated by people's tendency to perceive those who disagree with them as biased. Third, these studies manipulate the mediator and show that experimental manipulations that prompt people to perceive adversaries as biased lead them to respond more conflictually-and that such responding causes those who engage in it to be viewed as more biased and less worthy of cooperative gestures. In summary, this article provides evidence for a "bias-perception conflict spiral," whereby people who disagree perceive each other as biased, and those perceptions in turn lead them to take conflict-escalating actions against each other (which in turn engender further perceptions of bias, continuing the spiral).  相似文献   

19.
Over the past few decades, Indigenous communities have successfully campaigned for greater inclusion in decision-making processes that directly affect their lands and livelihoods. As a result, two important participatory rights for Indigenous peoples have now been widely recognized: the right to consultation and the right to free, prior and informed consent (FPIC). Although these participatory rights are meant to empower the speech of these communities—to give them a proper say in the decisions that most affect them—we argue that the way these rights have been implemented and interpreted sometimes has the opposite effect, of denying them a say or ‘silencing’ them. In support of this conclusion we draw on feminist speech act theory to identify practices of locutionary, illocutionary, and perlocutionary group silencing that arise in the context of consultation with Indigenous communities.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号