首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 31 毫秒
1.
The tendency of food-deprived, protein-deprived, and sodium-deprived Norway rats (Rattus norvegicus) and their respective controls to affiliate with conspecifics deprived of either food, protein, or sodium was examined. The authors found that (a) independent of internal state, focal rats offered a forced choice between protein-deprived and protein-replete target rats spent more time near replete than deprived target rats; and (b) both food-deprived and sodium-deprived focal rats offered a forced choice between food-deprived and replete target rats spent less time near fasted rats than did well-fed and sodium-replete focal rats. The data indicate that (a) rats can distinguish both food-deprived and protein-deprived rats from replete rats and (b) the deprivation states of rats can affect their willingness to affiliate with deprived conspecifics.  相似文献   

2.
The effect of environmental enrichment on conditioned freezing to contextual cues in adult Sprague-Dawley rats was examined. The freezing of both enriched-and standard-reared rats increased with the time spent in the chamber prior to shock. Both groups of rats showed equally low levels of contextual conditioning following a preshock period of 4 s and equally high levels following a 120-s preshock period. However, following a preshock period of 16 s enriched rats displayed more contextual conditioning than standard rats. That is, enriched rats appeared to process contextual information faster than their standard-reared counterparts. Enriched- reared rats also showed a greater ability to discriminate between the conditioning context and a similar but distinctive context. Hence, in addition to forming a representation of the context in memory more rapidly than standard-reared rats, enriched-reared rats also appear to form a more complex representation.  相似文献   

3.
朱熊兆  彭素芳  张晟  张逸  蔡琳 《心理学报》2012,44(3):330-337
为研究慢性温和应激诱导的抑郁大鼠纹状体内前列腺凋亡反应蛋白(prostate apoptosis response-4, par-4)的表达, 及甲基化是否参与par-4基因表达的调控, 将10周龄大鼠随机分为实验组和对照组, 实验组接受慢性温和应激, 对照组不接受实验性处理。于大鼠13周龄时, 采用强迫游泳、糖水偏爱测验测定大鼠的抑郁水平, 以实时定量PCR检测纹状体par-4及多巴胺D2受体(Dopamine receptor D2, DRD2) mRNA表达水平, 免疫印迹法检测纹状体par-4蛋白质表达水平, 用亚硫酸盐测序法检测par-4基因启动子区甲基化水平。结果发现, 与对照组大鼠相比, 实验组大鼠漂浮时间延长, 糖水偏爱率降低, 脑纹状体par-4、DRD2 mRNA及par-4蛋白质表达水平均降低, par-4基因启动子区甲基化水平两组差异不显著。提示慢性温和应激诱导大鼠产生了抑郁样行为, 并能抑制纹状体par-4基因的表达, 而基因甲基化可能并不参与其调控机制。  相似文献   

4.
Spontaneously hypertensive rats (SHRs) and the normotensive Wistar/Kyoto (WKY) rat were observed, along with Wistar rats (which represent the parent strain), on various open-field behaviors. All three strains were subsequently exposed to the activity-stress (A-S) ulcerogenic procedure. SHR and Wistar rats were very active in most open-field measures as compared with WKY rats, but only SHRs were active during the A-S treatment. WKY rats were very ulcer prone and had significantly more ulcers than SHRs, which in turn had more ulcers than Wistar rats. It was anticipated that Wistar rats would resemble the WKY rats, but in most measures the Wistars resembled the SHRs. The study suggests that although WKY rats function as an appropriate control for hypertension studies, these rats may be inappropriate as controls for other physiological and behavioral studies.  相似文献   

5.
The effects of crowding on thyrotropin (TSH) and somatotropin (GH) secretion were studied in two-month-old male rats. Crowded rats (9-10 per cage) showed lower serum GH levels than controls (3 per cage). Likewise, serum GH was lower in crowded rats after acute exposure to stress. However, percentage inhibition of GH secretion induced by acute stress was similar in crowded and control rats. Crowding reduced the TSH response to acute stress. The results found with the administration of hypothalamic regulatory factors suggest that the impaired GH and TSH secretion observed in crowded rats was not likely to be at the pituitary level. Therefore, altered neuroendocrine control of GH and TSH secretion appears to exist in crowded rats. Preliminary results obtained in rats crowded from weaning to adulthood suggest that food restriction only partially accounts for the changes observed in crowded rats.  相似文献   

6.
The aim of the present experiment was to study the effects of a low dose (0.001 mg/kg) of guanfacine, alpha-2 agonist, on the acquisition and retention of a water maze task measuring spatial reference memory in young and aged rats. Aged rats were impaired in the acquisition of this task. Both young and aged rats treated with guanfacine had shorter escape latencies than their saline treated counterparts. However, guanfacine treatment increased the speed of swimming in aged rats. According to the results of the probe trial, guanfacine may slightly improve the acquisition/retention of water maze task in young rats, whereas it may slightly impair the acquisition/retention of aged rats. The results suggest that a low dose of guanfacine administered peripherally may have different effects on young and aged rats in water maze performance, and a low dose of guanfacine does not improve spatial reference memory in aged rats.  相似文献   

7.
The anorexic consequence of thiamin deprivation was investigated in ventromedial hypothalamic (VMH) hyperphagic rats under either high-fat or low-fat thiamin-free diet conditions. The low-fat diet maintained feeding significantly longer in thiamin-deprived VMH rats than in intact rats, whereas the hig-fat diet sustained feeding in thiamin-deficient intact rats and accelerated anorexia onset in vitamin B1 deprived VMH rats. This effect was noted under both ad lib and pair-feeding conditions. Thiamin-deprived VMH rats subjected to weight control developed anorexia sooner than intact subjects regardless of the diet employed. The VMH rats fed a high-fat diet failed to resume feeding after thiamin readministration, which was interpreted as a permanent aversion to this diet. The relation between dietary intake and conditioned taste aversion is discussed with reference to the VMH and intact rat.  相似文献   

8.
Objectmarking and object sniffing were assessed in an open field during six 5-min trials in unoperated Long-Evans rats and rats with ibotenate lesions of the hippocampus and/or neocortex. Object marking was higher in hippocampally lesioned rats than in unoperated rats. Object marking did not differ between neocortically lesioned rats and unoperated rats. Object sniffing durations and visits did not differ between unoperated and hippocampally lesioned rats nor between unoperated and neocortically lesioned rats. A new object elicited longer sniffing by both unoperated and hippocampally lesioned rats. Neocortically lesioned rats did not show this effect. There were no effects of the new object on marking. Computerized tracing of open field paths revealed a smaller perimeter track for hippocampally lesioned rats than for unoperated rats. This difference reflected distinct ambulatory patterns. Hippocampally lesioned rats made stereotyped hind-limb pivots at each corner, while normal rats used forelimb pivots or reared and reoriented adjacent to the wall. Rearing was lower in hippocampally lesioned rats, and higher in neocortically lesioned rats, than in unoperated rats. These data indicate that investigative object behavior (sniffing) is resistant to the effects of hippocampal damage, while object-elicited marking and motoric output may be profoundly altered. The data on sniffing suggest either that (1) the noticeability of the objects used elicited investigative behaviors in hippocampally damaged rats comparable to those of novelty-induced exploration in normal rats or (2) object exploration is not used to create a spatial map, and, hence, not disturbed by hippocampal loss. Object marking may require spatial locale information to be exercised normally, or may index the mediation of an olfactory-modulated behavioral pattern through the hippocampal system.  相似文献   

9.
In the first of two experiments, three cotton rats (Sigmodon hispidus) and three albino rats were exposed to instrumental escape, unsignaled avoidance, and signaled avoidance, in that order. All subjects learned the escape procedure quickly, with the albino rats having generally shorter latencies, higher response rates, and requiring fewer sessions to reach the criterion. When the avoidance contingency was introduced, the cotton rats continued to respond almost entirely in the presence of the shock, whereas the albino rats responded in its absence, thus displaying effective avoidance behavior. Introduction of a pre-aversive stimulus did not improve the performance of the cotton rats. In the second experiment, five cotton rats and four albino rats were exposed to a free-operant (Sidman) avoidance procedure with a shock-shock interval of 3 sec and a response-shock interval of 20 sec. The cotton rats initiated responding at lower shock intensities than the albino rats, but their asymptotic avoidance responding was far less effective.  相似文献   

10.
This study was undertaken to confirm previous findings that Long-Evans rats exhibit age-related changes in the diurnal/nocturnal distribution of water intake and to examine the circadian pattern of these age-related changes. Twenty-one aged and 10 young pathogen-free rats were continuously monitored for water consumption over 12:12 h light/dark cycles. ANOVA, profile analysis, and cosinor analysis each demonstrated that aged rats differed from young rats. The age-related changes in circadian pattern can be described as a blunted rhythm (decreased amplitude) and an altered timing of peak activity (advanced acrophase). These differences, however, were only apparent in a subset of aged rats with the remaining aged rats exhibiting a circadian pattern indistinguishable from that of the young group.  相似文献   

11.
Pairs of rats received grid shock in a conical enclosure. Each shock elicited the stereotyped fighting posture. An omnidirectional pole, which was always between the rats, was accidentally operated whenever the rats fought. The number of pole hits, and the number of observed fight bouts, increased with current intensity. The number of pole hits served as a reliable and objective measure of shock-induced fighting in rats.  相似文献   

12.
Abstract:  To examine the effects of arachidonic acid (AA) on age-related cognitive deficits, Fischer 344 rats were given an AA-supplemented powder diet for 12 weeks from 18.6 months old as part of the old AA (OA) group. Other age-matched rats were fed a non-AA-supplemented powder diet as part of the old control (OC) group, and young rats were fed lab pellets as part of the young control (YC) group. When the aged rats reached 20.0 months old, all animals were tested for spontaneous activity in an open field, followed by the Morris water maze place and cue tasks. Escape latencies for the place task showed that the YC rats were fastest, and there was no difference between the OA and OC groups. However, the probe test and the first block of the cue task indicated that the OA rats remembered the location of an invisible platform better than the OC rats. Although the amount of hippocampal AA was larger in the OA rats than in the OC rats, the difference was not significant. The results showed that AA administration to aged animals may alleviate age-related deficits in spatial cognition.  相似文献   

13.
In the first part of the experiment, characteristics of T-maze alternation behavior were compared by giving one group of 11 rats seven sessions under conditions of "rerun correction" (contingent reinforcement for alternation) and another group of 11 rats the same number of sessions with noncontingent reinforcements, i.e., entering either goal arm of the T-maze was reinforced. Both groups alternated significantly above chance, and the difference in medians between groups was small but significant in later sessions. However, rats given rerun correction training quickly came against the "ceiling" of 100% alternation, and individual differences were small. The scores of rats run under conditions of "spontaneous" alternation were more variable, both within and between rats. In the second part of the experiment, six rats in each group received small electrolytic lesions in the posterodorsal septum. There were five operated-control rats from each group. After 2-wk postoperative recovery all rats were again given seven sessions in the T-maze. Under rerun correction procedure (contingent reinforcement), the controls retained near-perfect alternation scores. Most animals with lesions dropped to chance levels but recovered to control level over the seven sessions. Rats tested under spontaneous alternation conditions behaved quite differently. The control group continued to alternate significantly, but rats with lesions significantly alternated less and did not recover.  相似文献   

14.
探讨右旋柠烯(D-L)对癫痫幼鼠海马星形胶质细胞胶质原纤维酸性蛋白(GFAP)的影响。利用尼氏染色及免疫组化技术观察匹罗卡品致癫痫的幼鼠经药物(左乙拉西坦、丙戊酸钠、D-L)治疗后星形胶质细胞形态学变化和GFAP的表达。尼氏染色显示药物治疗后幼鼠海马胶质神经元脱失较未治疗的幼鼠明显减轻;海马GFAP免疫阳性细胞数在药物治疗后的幼鼠较未治疗幼鼠明显减少(P〈O.05)。幼鼠癫痫发作可能与海马星形胶质细胞增生有关,D-L对发育期大鼠颞叶癫痫星形胶质细胞的增生及GFAP的表达似有抑制作用,推测D-L可能具有抗癫痫作用。  相似文献   

15.
The behavioral economics of production.   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1       下载免费PDF全文
In two experiments, thirsty rats licked an empty spout instrumentally for water delivered at a neighboring spout. Each such pair of spouts constituted a work station, and one, two, or three stations were available in the test enclosure. In 1-hr sessions, the rats worked alone or in the company of 1 or 2 other rats, and performed either five, 10, or 40 licks at the empty spout for each water delivery. The total number of empty-spout licks, summed across rats and stations, increased with the empty-lick requirement and, with some exceptions, the number of rats in the enclosure and the number of work stations available. A Cobb-Douglas production function, with instrumental responding as an output and the three independent variables as inputs, accounted for a significant percentage of the variance. Contrary to that function, output failed to increase with additional rats (or work stations) when the number of work stations (or rats) was relatively small.  相似文献   

16.
The goal of the study was to provide evidence that the production of 50-kHz calls by adult rats is driven by potential or direct social contacts. The calls have been studied during daily visits to a cage by single or paired rats. Repeated exposure of rats to the cage frequently visited by other rats or direct contact between rats significantly increased the number of 50-kHz calls. The increase in production of 50-kHz calls was reduced by 78% after intrapreoptic-anterior hypothalamic injection of MK-801, an N-methyl-D-aspartate-type glutamate receptor antagonist. Calls emitted in all situations had a similar acoustic profile. It was found that 50-kHz calls were produced in anticipation of, and/or during, direct social contacts among adult rats and were predominantly initiated by olfactory stimuli. The calls seem to express an appetitive behavioral state in which the central glutamatergic mechanism is implicated.  相似文献   

17.
Laboratory rats, like many other animal species, transport food. Their behavior is influenced by factors such as food size, the time required to eat, travel distance, travel difficulty, and the availability of cover, etc. Recent versions of optimal foraging theory suggest that species that display such behavioral patterns do so in order to minimize risk to predation while at the same time maximizing food gain. Nevertheless, it is not clear that this explanation applies to rats, nor is it easy to investigate this problem in a laboratory. The results of the present study on urban feral rats show that their food handling behavior is similar to that of domestic rats. The results also suggest that food carrying can serve defensive as well as communicative functions. Aggression around food sources was high. Smaller rats always carried food and some large rats infrequently carried food, suggesting that the food carrying by smaller, subordinate rats may help them avoid conspecific aggression. The rats also vigorously attempted to steal food that was carried home by conspecifics. This result suggests that food carrying can redistribute food resources and inform conspecifics about food availability. The results demonstrate the utility of multilevel behavioral analysis and also demonstrate that for rats, food transport has functions other than predator avoidance, including avoidance of conspecific aggression and communication about food availability. © 1996 Wiley-Liss, Inc.  相似文献   

18.
Maudsley reactive (MR/Har) and nonreactive (MNRA/Har) rats (Rattus norvegicus) were tested in successive, simultaneous, and anticipatory contrast procedures. The MR/Har rats showed smaller successive negative contrast effects than the MNRA/Har rats when shifted from 32% to 4% sucrose, and the degree of contrast was smaller in animals of both strains than that typically obtained with unselected Sprague-Dawley derived rats. Chlordiazepoxide (4 and 8 mg/kg), which typically reduces contrast, did not influence degree of contrast in rats of either strain. Animals of both strains showed positive and negative contrast in the simultaneous contrast procedure, but degree of contrast in both cases was smaller in rats of the MR/Har strain. Animals of both strains also showed anticipatory contrast when a 0.15% saccharin solution preceded 32% sucrose in once-per-day pairings. In terms of latency to initiate licking, the MNRA/Har rats showed a contrast effect, but the MR/Har rats showed a "reinforcement" effect--shorter latency when saccharin preceded sucrose than when saccharin preceded saccharin. Open-field tests showed typical strain differences: The MNRA/Har rats ambulated more, reared more, defecated less, and showed less thigmotaxis than the MR/Har rats.  相似文献   

19.
The antinociceptive effect of daily acute intrathecal morphine injections on the tail-flick withdrawal response was compared in Intact rats and rats that were spinally transected 3 to 4 weeks prior to morphine administration (Spinal rats). Spinal rats became tolerant to repeated intrathecal injections of 5, 15, or 30 micrograms of morphine within 3 days. Intact rats were not tolerant to these doses after 4 daily injections. Spinal rats, made tolerant to repeated intrathecal morphine injections, were not tolerant to a subcutaneous injection of 6.0 mg/kg of morphine. These data suggest that, in intact animals, tolerance to the antinociceptive effect of spinal morphine is modulated by descending, supraspinal input.  相似文献   

20.
Rats with septal or hippocampal lesions, relative to normal rats, showed facilitated acquisition of a shuttle box avoidance response. The rats with septal lesions were also highly resistant to extinction compared with normal rats. When the same lesion effects were examined in hypophysectiomized rats, the animals with septal lesions continued to show facilitated performance, and those rats with hippocampal lesions performed no differently than nonoperated control animals. These findings are consistent with the hypothesis that the facilitated avoidance performance found in rats with hippocampal lesions is attributable to lesion-induced changes in hypophyseal activity, but similar changes induced by septal lesions are not.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号