首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
恐惧的过度泛化是焦虑障碍的核心症状之一, 表现为患者对与原危险刺激极不相似的中性刺激也有着较高强度的恐惧反应。临床上, 女性比男性更有可能患焦虑障碍, 因而对恐惧泛化进行性别差异研究可以为解释女性有着更高焦虑障碍发病率提供新的角度, 同时为临床治疗提供参考。本研究采用辨别性条件恐惧范式, 以主观预期值和皮电反应值作为测量指标, 从行为和生理两个层面对条件性恐惧泛化程度和恐惧泛化消退的性别差异进行研究。结果发现, 在恐惧泛化程度上, 未出现显著性别差异。在恐惧泛化消退上, 在主观预期值和皮电反应值两个层面均有着显著性别差异, 具体表现为相较于男性, 女性恐惧泛化的消退更慢, 持续时间更长。研究结果表明, 女性焦虑障碍高发病率的潜在影响因素之一可能在于女性对于恐惧泛化刺激的难以消除。  相似文献   

2.
3.
4.
Three experiments explored the issue of selective associations in the observational conditioning of fear. Experiment 1 results indicated that observer rhesus monkeys acquired a fear of snakes through watching videotapes of model monkeys behaving fearfully with snakes. In Experiment 2, observers watched edited videotapes that showed models reacting either fearfully to toy snakes and nonfearfully to artificial flowers (SN+/FL-) or vice versa (FL+/SN-). SN+/FL- observers acquired a fear of snakes but not of flowers; FL+/SN- observers did not acquire a fear of either stimulus. In Experiment 3, monkeys solved complex appetitive discriminative (PAN) problems at comparable rates regardless of whether the discriminative stimuli were the videotaped snake or the flower stimuli used in Experiment 2. Thus, monkeys appear to selectively associate snakes with fear.  相似文献   

5.
Epidemiological studies show that women are twice as likely as men to develop panic disorder (PD) during their lifetimes. Data from retrospective studies also suggest that women are more distressed by panic and other negative emotional states than men, and that this tendency may precede the development of PD. The present prospective study sought to expand this work by evaluating sex differences in the predisposition to panic in individuals without PD or other forms of psychopathology. Participants (N=96; 52 women) were exposed to 12 panicogenic inhalations of 20% CO2-enriched air while physiological (e.g., heart rate, electrodermal response, Frontalis EMG) and self-report (e.g., subject distress, fear, frequency and severity of DSM-IV panic symptoms) response domains were monitored. As expected, magnitude of autonomic responding failed to reliably discriminate between women and men. Yet, women reported more fear and panic immediately following the challenge procedure relative to men, and this sex difference persisted when assessed again 30 min post-challenge. Implications of these results are discussed broadly in the context of biopsychosocial accounts of sex differences in the clinical presentation of PD.  相似文献   

6.
Research has shown that emotional avoidance and anxiety sensitivity are associated with more self-reported fear and distress in response to laboratory fear challenge procedures. The present study aimed to expand upon this work and examined how emotional avoidance and anxiety sensitivity are related to emotional and physiological responses to an observational fear challenge procedure. To accomplish this aim, a carefully screened, non-clinical sample (N = 43) was administered the Acceptance and Action Questionnaire (AAQ), a measure of emotional avoidance, and the Anxiety Sensitivity Index (ASI). Participants then engaged in an observational fear challenge paradigm. During the fear challenge, participants watched mock panic attacks while emotional (e.g., fear and panic) and skin conductance levels were assessed. Consistent with expectation, emotional avoidance and anxiety sensitivity were positively associated with more self-reported fear and more severe panic symptoms to the challenge procedure. However, anxiety sensitivity was more highly associated with self-reported fear and panic symptoms in response to the challenge procedure than emotional avoidance. Emotional avoidance and anxiety sensitivity were not associated with levels of physiological arousal to the observational fear challenge procedure. Discussion focuses on the interplay between emotional avoidance, anxiety sensitivity, and the development of vicarious fear responses and how these constructs may contribute to the pathogenesis of anxiety disorders.  相似文献   

7.
8.
9.
In the present experiment, sex differences in hemispheric asymmetry during classical conditioning to emotional stimuli are reported. 125 subjects (62 females and 63 males) were shown a slide of a happy face in the right (or left) visual half field (VHF), and simultaneously a slide of an angry face in the left (or right) VHF. Eight groups were formed by the combination of male and female subjects; left and right VHF positions of the angry/happy faces; and the administration/omission of the shock unconditioned stimulus (UCS). Dependent measures were skin conductance responses recorded from both hands. The results during extinction showed a significant larger SCR magnitude to the shock compared to the no-shock groups only for the female subjects. CS position during conditioning was also important in revealing differential responding to either the happy or angry faces. A right hemisphere effect was found for the angry face CS for both the male and female subjects, however with a greater difference for the females.  相似文献   

10.
Sex differences in the neurobiological mechanisms involved in fear conditioning and extinction have been suggested to contribute to differential vulnerability for the development of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) in women compared with men. Reproductive hormones, such as estradiol, have been shown to facilitate fear conditioning and extinction learning and may explain some of these differences. However, the effect of commonly used hormonal contraceptives on the neurobiological mechanisms of fear conditioning and extinction is poorly understood. A laboratory study was conducted in trauma-exposed men and women with and without full or partial PTSD to examine effects of sex and use of hormonal birth control on fear conditioning, fear extinction learning, and extinction retention. Participants underwent fear conditioning with stimuli that were paired (CS+) or unpaired (CS−) with shock. Extinction learning occurred 72 h later, and extinction retention was tested 1 wk after extinction. Women on hormonal contraceptives (HCs) demonstrated enhanced acquisition of fear conditioning and enhanced extinction of fear as compared with women off hormonal birth control and men. While clinical implications have yet to be determined, these results suggest that hormonal contraceptives may facilitate learning during both fear acquisition and extinction. Understanding the impact of sex and hormones on fear conditioning and extinction processes may lead to new insights into the pathophysiology of PTSD and result in advancements in treatment that may vary by sex.

It is widely recognized that posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is a common consequence of trauma exposure, and women are at particularly high risk, with some but not all studies finding that women develop PTSD at twice the rate of men, despite greater trauma exposure in men (Breslau et al. 1998; Tanielian et al. 2008). Although some have suggested that greater exposure to interpersonal violence may contribute to higher rates of PTSD in women, other evidence implicates sex differences in the neurobiological mechanisms that are involved in fear conditioning and extinction. Enhanced fear conditioning and diminished extinction of conditioned fear have been associated with higher levels of endogenous estrogen in women, as well as the development and maintenance of PTSD in both sexes (Orr et al. 2000; Milad et al. 2009b; Glover et al. 2012). Furthermore, as one of the most empirically supported treatments for PTSD is prolonged exposure therapy, which is largely based on fear extinction principles and the success of extinction learning (Rothbaum and Davis 2003), a clear understanding of the individual factors impacting fear conditioning and extinction is critical.Although some of the studies examining sex differences in fear conditioning are inconsistent (Guimaraes et al. 1991; Zorawski et al. 2005; Milad et al. 2006), an increasing body of evidence from rodent and human studies supports the existence of sex differences in fear extinction learning and recall (Maren et al. 1994; Pryce et al. 1999; Milad et al. 2009a; Merz et al. 2013). One possible explanation for the lack of consistent findings in the fear conditioning literature may be related to potential floor effects associated with subclinical impairment of nonclinical samples, as most laboratory studies examining sex differences were conducted in healthy humans. Another explanation that is gaining substantial support is the impact of hormones that differ between the sexes, among individuals, and even within individuals across time (Quirk and Mueller 2008; Lebron-Milad and Milad 2012; Arevalo et al. 2015; Hwang et al. 2015; Herrera et al. 2017; Maeng et al. 2017; Antov and Stockhorst 2018).The available literature indicates that estradiol, the primary estrogen in women during the childbearing years, is also present at overall lower concentrations in males and plays a large role in the fear conditioning and extinction differences observed between men and women (Gupta et al. 2001; Jasnow et al. 2006; Chang et al. 2009; Milad et al. 2009a, 2010; Zeidan et al. 2011; Maddox et al. 2018; Matsumoto et al. 2018; Carvalho et al. 2021). In both sexes, estradiol plays a variety of important functions in the brain, including the regulation of oxidative stress, inflammation, and gene expression, as well as in cognitive functions such as learning and memory (Hammoud et al. 2020). Estrogen receptors are found throughout brain regions that are important for fear conditioning and extinction processes (e.g., the amygdala, ventromedial prefrontal cortex, and hippocampus), likely via enhancements to learning and memory (Milad et al. 2008; Quirk and Mueller 2008; Lebron-Milad and Milad 2012). In women, as peripheral levels of estradiol vary over the course of the menstrual cycle, so do levels of estradiol in the brain (Arevalo et al. 2015). Estradiol has been shown to enhance memory consolidation across stages of fear conditioning, extinction, and retention (Lebron-Milad and Milad 2012). Most relevant to learning in PTSD, women with high levels of estradiol demonstrate enhanced memory formation in the presence of stress exposure (Herrera et al. 2017; Antov and Stockhorst 2018). In animal and human models, higher estradiol levels appear to facilitate acquisition of fear conditioning and extinction (Maeng et al. 2017). For example, women with high endogenous estradiol levels have enhanced responses in fear circuitry during fear conditioning, extinction, and recall as compared with men (Hwang et al. 2015). When phase of menstrual cycle has been taken into account, differences have been observed in conditioned fear responses and severity of PTSD symptoms. Specifically, when women are in the midluteal phase of the menstrual cycle (higher endogenous estradiol), they demonstrate a stronger positive relationship between SCR during fear conditioning and PTSD symptoms than women in the early follicular phase of menstruation (lower endogenous estradiol) (Carpenter et al. 2022). In the complimentary literature on the startle response, low-estradiol women demonstrated reduced discrimination between CS+ and CS− during fear conditioning and reduced inhibition of fear-potentiated startle during extinction and extinction recall, indicating less successful learning than their high-estradiol counterparts (Glover et al. 2012, 2013; Armbruster et al. 2018). This literature suggests that higher levels of estradiol relate to enhanced acquisition of associations between an unconditioned stimulus (UCS) and a conditioned stimulus (CS) during fear conditioning and enhanced extinction of this association during the extinction phase due to greater memory consolidation.A variety of factors can account for hormone differences in women, including menstrual phase, age, and use of hormonal birth control. Approximately 11%–20% of women aged 20–39 yr use oral contraceptives (OCs) (Daniels and Abma 2020). Commonly used OCs directly affect estradiol levels and hormonal fluctuation associated with the menstrual cycle. However, hormonal contraceptives (HCs) have received little attention in the fear conditioning literature. At the time of writing, we were unable to locate studies examining the impact of other HCs on fear conditioning circuitry, although these forms of birth control are increasingly popular among women, and evidence suggests that a variety of HCs has impacts on brain structure, function, and cognitive processes (Brønnick et al. 2020). Literature suggests that estradiol levels in women on HCs are typically low, similar to those of women in the early follicular phase of menstruation (Brynhildsen 2014). Although Hwang et al. (2015) did not demonstrate an effect of HCs on fear conditioning, many HCs contain ethinyl estradiol, which is synthetic estrogen that binds to estrogen receptors at high levels. Further research is needed to determine whether synthetic estrogen present in HCs impacts fear conditioning and fear extinction and contributes to associated sex differences, particularly in a highly sensitized population such as those with PTSD. In the current study, differences in fear conditioning, extinction, and retention were examined in women on hormonal birth control and in the early follicular phase of the menstrual cycle as compared with men to determine whether the synthetic hormones present in HCs confer any enhancement to these processes over and above women off of birth control with theoretically low levels of endogenous estradiol.Fear conditioning is measured by assessing the differential SCR to a conditioned stimulus (CS+) paired with an unconditioned stimulus (shock; UCS) and a stimulus unpaired with a shock (CS−). Greater acquisition of fear conditioning is evidenced by greater SCR response to the CS+ when compared with the CS−. Fear extinction refers to repeated exposure to the CS in the absence of the US, which results in diminishing reactivity to previously conditioned stimuli due to an inhibitory neural link that is formed (Myers and Davis 2007). Extinction is therefore operationalized by reduced discrimination of responding to the CS+ and CS− over time, and its retention is evidenced by a maintenance of low differential SCR in response to presentation of CS+ and CS− cues at follow-up.In previous work, our group examined sex differences in skin conductance responses to a fear conditioning paradigm in men and women with PTSD (Inslicht et al. 2013). In that sample, women were premenopausal and underwent conditioning during the follicular phase of the menstrual cycle. We found that women had greater differential fear acquisition compared with men. Other work has indicated that the effects of endogenous gonadal hormones on fear extinction are moderated by PTSD diagnosis, such that women with PTSD demonstrated impaired fear extinction during the midluteal phase of menstruation but not during the early follicular phase (Pineles et al. 2016b). The current study examines the effect of sex, HC use, and PTSD severity on fear conditioning, fear extinction, and extinction retention in medically healthy trauma-exposed premenopausal women on or off HCs and age-matched men across a range of PTSD symptom severity. We used a validated laboratory conditioning paradigm (Inslicht et al. 2021) that occurred over several days in which fear acquisition was separated from extinction by 72 h to avoid influencing consolidation of fear conditioning, and extinction retention was evaluated 1 wk after the fear extinction session to provide a test of durability of extinction over time.Given evidence for impaired fear extinction in PTSD, we hypothesized that participants high in PTSD (men and women combined) would have decreased fear extinction learning and extinction retention compared with those with low PTSD symptom scores. As estradiol appears to enhance learning during stress in women, we predicted that women on HCs would demonstrate higher differential SCR during acquisition and lower differential SCR during extinction and extinction retention than naturally cycling women in the early follicular phase of menstruation and men. Finally, we predicted a PTSD × sex interaction effect for extinction learning and retention; women on HCs with high levels of current PTSD would have enhanced acquisition but decreased extinction learning and retention compared with women with low levels of current PTSD and men.  相似文献   

11.
Evidence has suggested that women have greater emotional reactivity than men. However, it is unclear whether these differences in basic emotional responses are also associated with differences in emotional distractibility, and what the neural mechanisms that implement differences in emotional distractibility between women and men are. Functional MRI recording was used in conjunction with a working memory (WM) task, with emotional distraction (angry faces) presented during the interval between the memoranda and the probes. First, we found an increased impact of emotional distraction among women in trials associated with high-confidence responses, in the context of overall similar WM performance in women and men. Second, women showed increased sensitivity to emotional distraction in brain areas associated with “hot” emotional processing, whereas men showed increased sensitivity in areas associated with “cold” executive processing, in the context of overall similar patterns of response to emotional distraction in women and men. Third, a sex-related dorsal–ventral hemispheric dissociation emerged in the lateral PFC related to coping with emotional distraction, with women showing a positive correlation with WM performance in left ventral PFC, and men showing similar effects in the right dorsal PFC. In addition to extending to men results that have previously been reported in women, by showing that both sexes engage mechanisms that are similar overall in response to emotional distraction, the present study identifies sex differences in both the response to and coping with emotional distraction. These results have implications for understanding sex differences in the susceptibility to affective disorders, in which basic emotional responses, emotional distractibility, and coping abilities are altered.  相似文献   

12.
One hundred and twenty subjects competed in a reaction-time task similar to that of Taylor (1967). Subjects were randomly assigned to a white noise, fine, or control condition. In the fine and noise conditions programmed opponents administered increasing provocation to subjects over a series of 24 trials. Control subjects were not provoked. As predicted, males retaliated with higher levels of noise than did females, while there were no sex differences in the fine condition. Contrary to prediction, sex of opponent had no effect. Subjects in all conditions tended to view the task as competitive but to devalue their opponent only in the noise condition. The prevalent assumption of female passivity in the face of instigation was rejected. Instead a dichotomy was proposed that while females are less likely than males to reciprocate to physical provocation, they are just as likely to respond to provocation of a nonphysical nature.Parts of this article were presented at the meeting of the American Psychological Association, Chicago, September 1975. Critical comments by Edward Donnerstein and Arnold Kahn on an earlier draft of this article are gratefully acknowledged.  相似文献   

13.
Fear conditioning, including variants such as delay and trace conditioning that depend on different neural systems, is widely used to behaviorally characterize genetically altered mice. We present data from three strains of mice, C57/BL6 (C57), 129/SvlmJ (129), and a hybrid strain of the two (F(1) hybrids), trained on various versions of a trace fear-conditioning protocol. The initial version was taken from the literature but included unpaired control groups to assess nonassociative effects on test performance. We observed high levels of nonassociative freezing in both contextual and cued test conditions. In particular, nonassociative freezing in unpaired control groups was equivalent to freezing shown by paired groups in the tests for trace conditioning. A number of pilot studies resulted in a new protocol that yielded strong context conditioning and low levels of nonassociative freezing in all mouse strains. During the trace-CS test in this protocol, freezing in unpaired controls remained low in all strains, and both the C57s and F(1) hybrids showed reliable associative trace fear conditioning. Trace conditioning, however, was not obtained in the 129 mice. Our findings indicate that caution is warranted in interpreting mouse fear-conditioning studies that lack control conditions to address nonassociative effects. They also reveal a final set of parameters that are important for minimizing such nonassociative effects and demonstrate strain differences across performance in mouse contextual and trace fear conditioning.  相似文献   

14.
Sex differences in response to the odor of alpha androstenone   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
Additional analyses of ratings by 98 male and 102 female college students assigned to one of four odor conditions showed no significant correlations for the methyl anthranilate or skatole odor conditions, but the pattern of correlations for men and women was different in the alpha androstenone condition, e.g., if men found alpha androstenone pleasant they tended to rate the target male positively.  相似文献   

15.
It has long been recognized that humans vary in their conditionability, yet the factors that contribute to individual variation in emotional learning remain to be delineated. The goal of the present study was to investigate the relationship among sex, stress hormones, and fear conditioning in humans. Forty-five healthy adults (22 females) underwent differential delay conditioning, using fear-relevant conditioned stimuli and a shock unconditioned stimulus. Salivary cortisol samples were taken at baseline and after acquisition training and a 24-h-delayed retention test. The results showed that acquisition of conditioning significantly correlated with postacquisition cortisol levels in males, but not in females. This sex-specific relationship was found despite similar overall levels of conditioning, unconditioned responding, and cortisol. There was no effect of postacquisition cortisol on consolidation of fear learning in either sex. These findings have implications for the understanding of individual differences in fear acquisition and risk factors for the development of affective disorders.  相似文献   

16.
In two conditioned suppression experiments, rats received Pavlovian forward defense conditioning in which tonal conditioned stimuli (CSs) terminated with the onset of scrambled grid shock unconditioned stimuli (USs). After this experience, the rats then received a Pavlovian backward conditioning procedure in which the same USs now terminated with the onset of the same CSs. Although the two experiments differed greatly in terms of CS and US parameters, number of forward and backward pairings, and in terms of the general techniques used to establish and measure the Pavlovian conditioned response (CR), the results of both experiments agreed in showing that backward conditioning can indeed weaken a CR based on forward pairings. The results also show that, under some conditions, the backward procedure can be at least as effective in weakening an established CR as the traditional CS-alone extinction procedure; but, under other conditions, the backward procedure is less effective and leads to more spontaneous recovery than the CS-alone procedure.  相似文献   

17.
Animal Cognition - This research focuses on sex differences in the behavioral patterns of dogs when they are exposed to human chemosignals (sweat) produced in happy and fear contexts. No age, breed...  相似文献   

18.
Six experiments used rats to study blocking and unblocking of fear learning. An excitatory stimulus (A) blocked fear learning to a neutral stimulus (B). Unblocking of B occurred if the AB compound signaled an increase in unconditioned stimulus (US) intensity or number. Assessments of associative change during blocking showed that more was learned about B than A. Such assessments during unblocking revealed that more was learned about B than A following an increase in US intensity but not US number. These US manipulations had no differential effects on single-cue learning. The results show that variations in US intensity or number produce unblocking of fear learning, but for each there is a different profile of associative change and a potentially different mechanism.  相似文献   

19.
The authors propose that a multimodal classical conditioning model be considered when clinicians or clinical researchers study the etiology of fears and anxieties learned by human beings. They argue that fears can be built through the combined effects of direct, observed, and verbally presented classical conditioning trials. Multimodal classical conditioning is offered as an alternative to the three pathways to fear argument prominent in the human fear literature. In contrast to the three pathways position, the authors present theoretical arguments for why "learning by observation" and "learning through the receipt of verbal information" should be considered classical conditioning through observational and verbal modes. The paper includes a demonstration of how data, commonly collected in research on the three pathways to fear, would be studied differently using a multimodal classical conditioning perspective. Finally, the authors discuss implications for assessment, treatment, and prevention of learned fears in humans.  相似文献   

20.
Three experiments using a conditioned punishment paradigm with rat subjects examined the possibility that the nonmonotonic acquisition function previously found to characterize simultaneous conditioning was due to the noninformative nature of the conditioned stimulus (CS). In Experiment 1 the suppressive effects of a CS previously presented with an unconditioned stimulus (US) in a simultaneous and forward (informative) manner were compared following 20 and an additional 60 conditioning trials. Excitatory conditioning similarly diminished with increased trials for both the simultaneous and forward procedures. Experiment 2 employed a between-groups design. Simultaneous, forward, and trace conditioning procedures were compared following 20 or 100 trials. Each of the three 100-trial groups showed less resistance to extinction than their 20-trial counterparts. Experiment 3 determined that the decrement in excitatory conditioning for the 100-trial groups was not due to the greater number of US presentations, per se, but rather to the number of CS-US pairings. The nonmonotonic acquisition function observed with all three conditioning procedures indicated that informational factors were not responsible for the decrement observed in simultaneous conditioning. The pattern of results suggested that subjects receiving extended conditioning trials were better able to discriminate between training and testing.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号