共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
Elisabeth Schürer-Necker 《Integrative psychological & behavioral science》1990,25(4):195-200
Walker and Tarte (1963) postulate that at short retention intervals high arousal paired-associates are reproduced more poorly
than low arousal items. Walker and colleagues believe that this hypothesis is confirmed by their paired-associate learning
studies. However, results of these paired-associate learning studies are position confounded artifacts. Better recall of low
arousal items at short-term retention is caused by the coincidence of the recency effect and low arousal at the end of the
trial. When these position effects are controlled there is no action decrement for the high arousal paired-associates. To
test this assumption, the Kleinsmith and Kaplan study (1963) was replicated and two other variations were conducted. In these
three studies with 76 subjects, which were tested at two minutes or 1 week, the action decrement occurs only when the two
position effects coincide. 相似文献
7.
8.
9.
Andrew R. Fuller 《International journal of psychology》1978,13(2):123-128
Eysenck has theorized that introverts are high in arousal and should show the pattern of results which has been displayed in several experiments by subjects confronted with high-arousal stimulus materials: poor performance on short-term and good performance on long-term measures of retention. Extraverts, on the other hand, are thought to be low in arousal and should show the reverse pattern as displayed by subjects confronted with low-arousal stimulus materials. The relevant experiments conducted in Britain confirm these predictions. The relevant United States studies do not. The method used in the present study, in which Americans were subjects, is that of an experiment of Kleinsmith and Kaplan (1964) which found the patterns of retention predicted on the basis of the subjects' exposure to high- or low-arousal stimulus materials. But, as in the other tests of Eysenck's predictions, arousal was measured in terms of introversion and extraversion. The results failed to support Eysenck's formulation. 相似文献
10.
11.
12.
13.
14.
15.
Ss were presented pairs of items on study cycles; on test cycles Ss were presented the stimulus items and required either to recall the correct response or to indicate the correct response from three alternatives. It was demonstrated that Ss who chose the correct responses from the alternatives tended to select from both stimulus and response compounds so that the association was between stimulus and response components. The amount of selection from the response compounds was comparable to that from the stimulus compounds. 相似文献
16.
Marcia Z. Lippman 《Memory & cognition》1974,2(2):385-390
Three experiments were conducted to study the effects of enactive imagery (EI) on associative learning. In Experiment I, groups of Ss rated 226 verbs on EI and frequency. In Experiments II and III, Ss learned a 24- and a 16-item list, respectively. The lists consisted of the four possible stimulus-response combinations of high (H) and low (L) EI verb pairs: H-H, H-L, L-H, L-L. In both experiments, EI was found to be a significant factor on the stimulus side, performance being superior when the stimulus was of high EI. In Experiment III, the response EI main effect and the Stimulus by Response EI interaction were also found to be significant. The results indicated that like the imagery evoked by nouns, the EI evoked by verbs facilitates learning. 相似文献
17.
18.
Mathematical models of memory can be evaluated using a continuous paired-associate learning paradigm in which a second response is requested immediately following an error. This procedure provides discriminability among the models on the basis of the relationship between the first- and second-response error probabilities. Theoretical predictions are derived for a variety of models, including finite-state and noncontingent operator models, formulations based on the theory of signal detectability, the multicomponent memory model of Bower (1967b), and the long- and short-term model of Atkinson and Shiffrin (1968). None of these is unequivocally acceptable, and many are decisively rejected by the data of a single-trial experiment. The most satisfactory models are the multicomponent model and the Atkinson and Shiffrin model, both of which assume a greater degree of internal structure than the rejected models. It appears that internal structure of this sort will be a necessary part of any satisfactory model of second responses. 相似文献
19.
20.