共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
2.
3.
4.
Christopher Perricone 《The Journal of value inquiry》1987,21(3):199-207
5.
6.
The location and interpretation of the bisection point 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
Lorraine G. Allan 《The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section B: Comparative and Physiological Psychology》2002,55(1):43-60
In atemporal bisectiontask with humans, the observer is required to decide whether a probe duration ( t ) is more similar to the short referent (S), an R S response, or to the long referent (L), an R L response. Temporal bisection yields a psychometric function relating the proportion of long responses, P(R L ), to probe duration t . The value of t at which R S and R L occur with equal frequency, P(RL) = .5, is referred to as the bisection point, T 1/2 . Bisection models usually interpret T 1/2 as identifying the value of t that is equally confusable with S and L, but they differ in their predictions for the location of T 1/2 . The present paper presents new data relevant to the location and interpretation of T 1/2 . The data indicate that the empirical values usually are biased, the biases being influenced by duration range, L:S ratio, and probe spacing. Moreover, the biases often are not consistent across observers. It is concluded that empirical values of T 1/2 should not be interpreted as indicating the value of t that is equally confusable with S and L. 相似文献
7.
Bisection tasks are used in research on normal space and time perception and to assess the perceptual distortions accompanying neurological disorders. Several variants of the bisection task are used, which often yield inconsistent results, prompting the question of which variant is most dependable and which results are to be trusted. We addressed this question using theoretical and experimental approaches. Theoretical performance in bisection tasks is derived from a general model of psychophysical performance that includes sensory components and decisional processes. The model predicts how performance should differ across variants of the task, even when the sensory component is fixed. To test these predictions, data were collected in a within-subjects study with several variants of a spatial bisection task, including a two-response variant in which observers indicated whether a line was transected to the right or left of the midpoint, a three-response variant (which included the additional option to respond “midpoint”), and a paired-comparison variant of the three-response format. The data supported the model predictions, revealing that estimated bisection points were least dependable with the two-response variant, because this format confounds perceptual and decisional influences. Only the three-response paired-comparison format can separate out these influences. Implications for research in basic and clinical fields are discussed. 相似文献
8.
V. Yu. Shavrukov 《Studia Logica》1991,50(1):143-148
We prove that the first order theory of the fixed point algebra corresponding to an r.e. consistent theory containing arithmetic is hereditarily undecidable. 相似文献
9.
An attempt is made to elaborate upon Lev's results concerning the reliability of the point biserial coefficient of correlation in a manner that will be helpful to the psychological statistician. Procedures required in the use of the non-centralt tables prepared by Johnson and Welch are described as they relate to the determination of the fiducial limits for a point biserial coefficient. A normal approximation technique for the estimation of fiducial limits is also suggested. Numerical evidence is presented which shows that relative to a given level of significance the width of the fiducial interval estimated from a point biserial coefficient of any size is smaller than that of the fiducial interval corresponding to an ordinary Pearsonian coefficient of the same magnitude. 相似文献
10.
Summary The study examines the possibility of observing on-line recognition of spoken words through manipulations of the location of the uniqueness point (UP) in a gender-classification task. The subjects were presented with spoken French nouns and had to indicate by a key-press response whether each was feminine or masculine. RTs measured from word onset were significantly correlated with UP location, a finding that supports the notion of on-line processing. The effect of UP location is, however, smaller than that predicted by the original cohort theory (that recognition occurs exactly at the UP). On the other hand, it is stronger when words with respectively early and late UPs are presented in homogeneous blocks rather than in mixed order. It is proposed that the results can be accounted for by the notion of a sub-optimal lexical strategy in which some monitoring of the phonetic data continues past the UP. 相似文献
11.
12.
13.
14.
15.
16.
R. Rorty uncouples cosmopolitanism from emancipation and rejects the latter on both phylogenetic and ontogenetic grounds. Thus: 1. There is no human nature to be emancipated, and 2. The notion of a rational, transcendental and conditioning subject (presupposed by traditional theories of emancipation) is obsolete. He preserves the idea of cosmopolitanism, which, in an effort to avoid foundationalisrn, he associates only with the development and progress of liberal societies. His cosmopolitanism relies on the distinction between persuasion and force and his preference for conversation over rational discourse. In this paper, I discuss Rorty's claims and trace residues of biologism, positivism, and behaviourism in them. By putting forward an immanent critique of Rorty's account of cosmopolitanism and emancipation, I defend a non-foundationalist notion of redemption as self-realization and propose a new justification of Rorty's distinction between force and persuasion. 相似文献
17.
Lubomir S. Prytulak 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1973,14(2):387-393
Data from two experiments indicate that (a) within 3.5 deg visual angle of the fixation point, an eccentrically fixated line and dot appear rectilinear when they are objectively convex toward the fixation point; (b) beyond 3.5 deg, they appear rectilinear when they are objectively concave; (c) the latter effect is not an artifact of the use of stationary fixation points; and (d) the addition of an extraneous line to the stimulus line so as to form a 90-deg angle acts as if to reduce the apparent size of the angle, but does not alter fixation point effects. The data were interpreted as supporting (a) the hypothesis that the stimulus line was more sensitive to illusory distortion than the dot, and (b) the ID (increasing-decreasing) model of the visual field—an inner area of increasing concentric contraction (stimuli appear to lie nearer the fixation point to a degree which increases with distance from the fixation point) is bounded by an outer area of decreasing concentric contraction. 相似文献
18.
J. A. Sheppard 《Theology & Science》2013,11(1):37-46
Many theological terms such as ‘causation’ and ‘creation’ carried technical meanings during the late thirteenth century but these same terms are often treated commonly in modern discourse. When the tension between creationism and evolution is examined in light of what well known medieval theologians wrote, it appears that the current debates related to evolution often lapse into the fallacy of equivocation. One unfortunate consequence of this breakdown is that the root of the problem, the subtle language involved, often remains unexposed. Moreover, the level of passionate discourse surrounding evolution suggests that a nearly scholastic approach is required if further confusion is to be limited. 相似文献
19.
Some researchers argue that task switching involves task-set reconfiguration-that is, changing the existing task set to perform
a different task. Although this idea is intriguing, it is often unclear what is reconfigured and which processes are involved
in reconfiguration. We addressed these issues by defining task sets, identifying differences between them, and obtaining evidence
that was diagnostic of reconfiguration. In two experiments, subjects performed relative judgment tasks that involved comparing
a target with a reference point that repeated or switched across trials. The task was the same on every trial, but the task
set was not, because a task-set parameter-the reference point-had to be changed. Target-reference point distance effects that
changed with the reference point provided diagnostic evidence that reconfiguration occurred, and this reconfiguration led
to switch costs. We discuss how our approach can be generalized to define reconfiguration more clearly in other task-switching
situations. nt|mis|This research was supported by Grants BCS 0133202 and BCS 0446806 from the National Science Foundation
to G.D.L. 相似文献
20.
Isabel Fraga Marc Guasch Juan Haro Isabel Padrón Pilar Ferré 《Behavior research methods》2018,50(1):84-93
We present here emoFinder (http://usc.es/pcc/emofinder), a Web-based search engine for Spanish word properties taken from different normative databases. The tool incorporates several subjective word properties for 16,375 distinct words. Although it focuses particularly on normative ratings for emotional dimensions (e.g., valence and arousal) and discrete emotional categories (fear, disgust, anger, happiness, and sadness), it also makes available ratings for other word properties that are known to affect word processing (e.g., concreteness, familiarity, contextual availability, and age of acquisition). The tool provides two main functionalities: Users can search for words matched on specific criteria with regard to the selected properties, or users can obtain the properties for a set of words. The output from emoFinder is highly customizable and can be accessed online or exported to a computer. The tool architecture is easily scalable, so that it can be updated to include word properties from new Spanish normative databases as they become available. 相似文献