首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 156 毫秒
1.
本研究旨在探讨智力落后儿童在实验情景下用手抓物的方式、及其与大脑侧偏化的关系。结果表明:(1)智力落后儿童混用手情况多。被试在取物时,使用左手或右手次数的比率平均约为1:2,相当于1—2岁正常儿童用左、右手取物的比率;(2)性别与年龄对落后儿童用手方式无明显影响;(3)病情等级对用手方式有明显影响:轻度智力落后儿童使用左、右手的比率大于中、重度智力落后儿童的同样比率;(4)智力落后儿童抓物的线索在四个实验中不完全相同:实验Ⅰ至Ⅲ的线索为位置(近手),实验Ⅳ为对实验用具的喜爱;(5)在混用手与大脑侧偏化(或优势脑未分化)不完善之间,可能有直接关系。此问题尚需进一步研究。  相似文献   

2.
周楠  方晓义 《心理科学》2011,34(3):714-722
心理理论是指对自己和他人心理状态(如需要、信念、意图、动机、感觉等)的认识,并由此对相应行为做出因果性的预测和解释。国内外心理理论研究较多关注一般儿童的心理理论能力,而对自闭症儿童领域的心理理论的研究不够。本研究在原有的错误信念任务的基础上,对任务进行完全“非言语”改进,以意外内容任务为主要测试内容,将智力落后儿童作为对照组纳入到实验当中,进一步探索自闭症儿童心理理论发展情况。研究结果表明:改编后的非言语意外内容任务适用于自闭症和智力落后儿童;包括低言语能力个体在内的所有自闭症儿童的心理理论能力显著低于智力落后儿童;智力落后儿童的心理理论能力与以往研究结果相一致;相对于智力落后儿童,自闭症儿童在物品转移和调换的注意方面存在更大障碍。  相似文献   

3.
智力落后儿童适应行为发展特点的研究   总被引:8,自引:0,他引:8  
张福娟 《心理科学》2002,25(2):170-172
本研究用儿童适应行为评定量表对4~12岁中、轻度智力落后儿童适应行为发展特点进行探讨,结果发现:他们的适应行为与智力有显著性相关;智力落后儿童适应行为发展落后,其中认知技能发展严重滞后是一个重要原因,但他们的适应行为仍有着相当的发展潜力。  相似文献   

4.
本研究通过对智力落后儿童和正常儿童听觉注意稳定性的比较研究,探索智力落后儿童听觉注意稳定性的特点及影响因素。结果表明:4岁智龄组智力落后儿童听觉注意稳定性较弱,低于或等于同智龄组正常儿童水平。智力落后儿童慢速和快速情境下的注意稳定性高于中速。信噪比也影响注意的稳定性,信噪比越低,注意稳定性越差。频率因素对智力落后儿童的影响不显著。  相似文献   

5.
视觉障碍儿童人格特征的比较研究   总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4  
张福娟  谢立波  袁东 《心理科学》2001,24(2):154-156
以《缺陷儿童人格诊断量表》为工具对视觉障碍儿童进行人格评定,并将其测验分数与先前有关其他特殊儿童和正常儿童的同一量表测验结果加以比较。比较结果表明.视觉障碍儿童与正常儿童、智力落后儿童的人格特性有明显差异,而与听觉障碍儿童的人格差异相对要小一些。随年龄、障碍程度的不同.视觉障碍儿童的人格特性也有明显差异。  相似文献   

6.
轻度智力落后学生心理健康问题的研究   总被引:7,自引:1,他引:6  
本研究使用心理健康诊断测验(MHT)手册对10-16岁(四至九年级)的轻度智力落后学生与智力正常学生间的心理健康水平进行比较研究,结果发现:轻度智力落后学生有不同程度的心理健康问题,随班就读轻度智力落后学生的心理健康问题尤为突出.有待相应有效的教育支持。  相似文献   

7.
智力落后学生句子判断能力的研究   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
以138名智力落后学生为被试,探讨了8-18岁学生的句子判断能力.结果显示:1)智力落后学生的句子判断能力随着年龄的增长而提高,在8-10岁到11-14岁之间增长显著,发展迅速;2)11-14岁组智力落后学生对于语法及语义错误的判断存在显著差异;3)句子判断能力相对较好的智力落后学生,因果关系复句与条件关系复句的判断成绩显著优于转折关系复句的判断成绩.  相似文献   

8.
儿童绘图作业内隐学习智力独立性研究   总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3  
实验采用中性参数程序和绘图作业,让正常智力、轻度智力落后、中度智力落后3组8~12岁右利手儿童接受符合、不符合或与“起始-旋转法则”无关的绘图练习,考察练习对原初行为方式的影响。结果发现,违反“起始-旋转法则”的练习使智力落后组被试的绘图行为发生了明显改变,证明内隐学习具有智力独立性;“意识污染”对内隐学习结果的影响可能是复杂的而非单向的;中性参数实验程序能够更好地隐蔽规则学习的实验目的,但不能有效消除意识污染的影响。  相似文献   

9.
超常儿童的创造力及其与智力的关系   总被引:5,自引:0,他引:5  
施建农  徐凡 《心理科学》1997,20(5):468-468,,477,
1问题提出在超常儿童研究领域中存在着许多有争议的问题,超常儿童的创造力(Creativity)与智力的关系问题就是其中的一个。一方面,越来越多的研究者认识到,创造力是儿童超常表现的一个重要方面,是构成超常特征的关键成份,是智力的一个维度。另一方面,许多相关性研究的结果却表明,智力超常儿童的创造力与其智力之间很少相关或相关很低.对此,通常有两种解释。一种是,当智力水平达到某一程度后(如IQ>120).创造力与智力可能表现为完全不同的两码事,或者说创造力是独立于智力的.另一种解释认为,目前的大多数创造力测验和智力…  相似文献   

10.
智力落后的成因--当代智力理论新解   总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4  
钱文 《心理科学》2004,27(6):1438-1441
智力落后的成因历来存在争论。本文在当代复杂系统智力理论的视野之下.从Sternberg的三元智力理论和Das的PASS模型入手,分析了智力落后本质及成因的新解.揭示了智力落后理论研究的认知心理学新走向。  相似文献   

11.
The lexical–semantic organization of the mental lexicon is bound to change across the lifespan. Nevertheless, the effects of lexical–semantic factors on word processing are usually based on studies enrolling young adult cohorts. The current study aims to investigate to what extent age-specific semantic organization predicts performance in referential word production over the lifespan, from school-age children to older adults. In Study 1, we conducted a free semantic association task with participants from six age-groups (ranging from 10 to 80 years old) to compute measures that capture age-specific properties of the mental lexicon across the lifespan. These measures relate to lifespan changes in the Available Richness of the mental lexicon and in the lexical–semantic Network Prototypicality of concrete words. In Study 2, we used the collected data to predict performance in a picture-naming task on a new group of participants within the same age-groups as for Study 1. The results show that age-specific semantic Available Richness and Network Prototypicality affect word production speed while the semantic variables collected only in young adults do not. A richer and more prototypical semantic network across subjects from a given age-group is associated with faster word production speed. The current results indicate that age-specific semantic organization is crucial to predict lexical–semantic behaviors across the lifespan. Similarly, these results also provide cues to the understanding of the lexical–semantic properties of the mental lexicon and to lexical selection in referential tasks.  相似文献   

12.
Investigating instances where lexical selection fails can lead to deeper insights into the cognitive machinery and architecture supporting successful word retrieval and speech production. In this paper, we used a multiplex lexical network approach that combines semantic and phonological similarities among words to model the structure of the mental lexicon. Network measures at different levels of analysis (degree, network distance, and closeness centrality) were used to investigate the influence of network structure on picture naming accuracy and errors by people with Anomic, Broca's, Conduction, and Wernicke's aphasia. Our results reveal that word retrieval is influenced by the multiplex lexical network structure in at least two ways—(a) the accuracy of production and error type on incorrect productions were influenced by the degree and closeness centrality of the target word, and (b) error type also varied in terms of network distance between the target word and produced error word. Taken together, the analyses demonstrate that network science techniques, particularly the use of the multiplex lexical network to simultaneously represent semantic and phonological relationships among words, reveal how the structure of the mental lexicon influences language processes beyond traditionally examined psycholinguistic variables. We propose a framework for how the multiplex lexical network approach allows for understanding the influence of mental lexicon structure on word retrieval processes, with an eye toward a better understanding of the nature of clinical impairments, like aphasia.  相似文献   

13.
Wei L 《Brain and language》2002,81(1-3):691-707
The Chinese/English intrasentential code-switching data provide evidence that the bilingual mental lexicon involves language contact between language-specific semantic/pragmatic feature bundles. Lemmas in the mental lexicon are tagged for specific languages and contain semantic, syntactic, and morphological information about lexemes. In a bilingual mode, the speaker makes choices at the preverbal level of lexical-conceptual structure, and these choices activate the lemmas in the mental lexicon for the speaker's preverbal message to be morphosyntactically realized at the functional level of predicate-argument structure. The result will be language-specific surface forms at the positional level of morphological realization patterns. The languages involved in the bilingual's mixed speech are never equally activated, with one language projecting the sentential frame and the other supplying a particular type of morphemes for the speaker's communicative intentions.  相似文献   

14.
We explore the features of a corpus of naturally occurring word substitution speech errors. Words are replaced by more imageable competitors in semantic substitution errors but not in phonological substitution errors. Frequency effects in these errors are complex and the details prove difficult for any model of speech production. We argue that word frequency mainly affects phonological errors. Both semantic and phonological substitutions are constrained by phonological and syntactic similarity between the target and intrusion. We distinguish between associative and shared-feature semantic substitutions. Associative errors originate from outside the lexicon, while shared-feature errors arise within the lexicon and occur when particular properties of the targets make them less accessible than the intrusion. Semantic errors arise early while accessing lemmas from a semantic-conceptual input, while phonological errors arise late when accessing phonological forms from lemmas. Semantic errors are primarily sensitive to the properties of the semantic field involved, whereas phonological errors are sensitive to phonological properties of the targets and intrusions.  相似文献   

15.
The present study investigated verbal recall of semantically preserved and degraded words and nonwords by taking into consideration the status of one's semantic short-term memory (STM). Two experiments were conducted on 2 Chinese individuals with aphasia. The first experiment showed that they had largely preserved phonological processing abilities accompanied by mild but comparable semantic processing deficits; however, their performance on STM tasks revealed a double dissociation. The second experiment found that the participant with more preserved semantic STM had better recall of known words and nonwords than of their unknown counterparts, whereas such effects were absent in the patient with severe semantic STM deficit. The results are compatible with models that assume separate phonological and semantic STM components, such as that of R. C. Martin, M. Lesch, and M. Bartha (1999). In addition, the distribution of error types was different from previous studies. This is discussed in terms of the methodology of the authors' experiments and current views regarding the nature of semantic STM and representations in the Chinese mental lexicon.  相似文献   

16.
Two experiments are reported on understanding complement clauses embedded into four Greek matrix verbs, Ask (ask information), Promise, Request and Tell (give information), equivalent to English in syntactic and semantic constraints. Experiment 1 was conducted with educably mentally retarded (EMR) and nonretarded (NR) children matched on verbal mental age (MA), and the second with fifth and sixth graders and young high school adults. The results suggest that: (1) EMR and NR children are similar in performance, despite other differences, according to Guttman coefficients of reproducibility and scalability, (2) verbal MA and Digit Span cannot best predict EMR and young NR children's linguistic behavior, (3) the minimal distance principle (MDP) advocated by Chomsky (1969, 1972, 1982) fails to account for performance by either population sample on constructions conforming to the same syntactic and semantic constraints as in English, (4) the semantic role principle (SRP) approach (Lederberg and Maratsos 1981; Maratsos 1974) falls short of making consistent predictions of performance by EMR and NR children on constructions requiring the same semantic role allocation, (5) evidence from Experiment 2, despite differences, supports the results of Experiment 1, (6) the results on understanding complement clauses of the type tested are discussed within the framework of the SRP approach in general, but the emphasis is placed on interaction of semantic and pragmatic presuppositions.  相似文献   

17.
Deep dyslexia is an acquired reading disorder that involves the production of semantic errors and the inability to read aloud nonwords successfully. Several explanations for this reading impairment posit multiple loci of damage to account for the various error types produced in deep dyslexia. In contrast, the failure of inhibition hypothesis suggests that damage in the phonological output lexicon alone can explain these errors. Specifically, this hypothesis proposes normal processing via orthographic and phonological reading routes, as well as an intact semantic system. However, slowed or reduced inhibitory connections result in the failure to suppress spuriously activated neighbours in the phonological output lexicon, where neighbourhood can be defined in terms of phonology, orthography, or semantics. Given a failure to inhibit semantically related candidates, semantic reading errors occur. Important to the test of this hypothesis is that it evolves several predictions that are contrary to performance observed in the normal population. In particular, semantic errors are predicted to be greater in conditions where words are blocked according to semantic category than in random presentations. In addition, a semantic interference effect is expected. The results of semantic blocking were consistent with these predictions and lend support to the failure of inhibition hypothesis.  相似文献   

18.
汉语的认知神经心理学研究   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
认知神经心理学为探讨语言的心理机制开辟了一条新途径,使得人们对心理词典的认识日渐深入。该文着重介绍了汉语方面的认知神经心理学研究成果,其中包括语义系统、语音输出词典、语音输出buffer的信息表征方式,以及汉字的书写机制与阅读机制  相似文献   

19.
We report the performance of LC, a deep dyslexic. We investigated extensively her errors according to serial cognitive neuropsychological models of oral reading. Initial evaluation of her reading suggested impaired access to the phonological output lexicon (POL). Impaired grapheme-to-phoneme conversion (GPC) and semantic errors in reading suggested that LC read via an impoverished semantic route. However, a serial model of oral reading could not explain error differences in reading, picture naming, spontaneous speech, and repetition. Neologisms occurred in oral reading but not in spontaneous speech and repetition. Semantic errors in naming exceeded those in oral reading. To account for these different error patterns we propose that the semantic route, the direct route from the orthographic input lexicon to the POL, and GPC activate simultaneously during reading, converging at the POL to constrain phonological selection. These routes are modular but not functionally encapsulated. For LC, the POL receives ambiguous information due to degradation of all routes, causing reading errors.  相似文献   

20.
高、低频汉字形音义激活的时间进程   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
采用基于字形的启动范畴判断任务,通过变化SOA和不同的启动类型,考察高、低频汉字形音义激活的相对时间进程。结果表明,高频汉字形音义激活的时间顺序为字形—字义—字音;低频汉字字形的激活在先,字音、字义的激活时间未能分开。这说明汉字的书面使用频率是影响汉字形、音、义激活时间进程的重要因素。本研究进一步支持了汉字形—音、形—义连接强度受汉字使用频率影响的观点。  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号