首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 156 毫秒
1.
采用自我同一性地位测定量表与自编中国大学生存在焦虑量表,以730名大学生为被试,研究了中国大学生自我同一性与存在焦虑关系。结果发现:(1)自我同一性中现在投入、将来投入愿望维度与存在焦虑显著正相关,过去危机与存在焦虑无显著相关。(2)不同类型的自我同一性地位大学生存在焦虑差异显著,其中自我同一性扩散型大学生的存在焦虑最高,自我同一性早闭型大学生存在焦虑最低。(3)自我同一性中过去危机、现在投入和将来投入愿望都能对存在焦虑产生显著预测力,但现在投入的负向预测力最为明显。相对于存在焦虑其他维度,自我同一性各因子对无意义与空虚焦虑的预测力最大。  相似文献   

2.
本研究旨在实验性地探究自我差异作为认知结构的存在性及其与情绪的关系.98名大学生参加了实验.实验Ⅰ考察自我差异类型与抑郁、焦虑水平的关系,结果表明:1)实际-理想、实际-应该自我差异均与焦虑存在显着相关,表明两种自我差异可作为焦虑水平的预测指标;2)中国被试的结果与美国研究结果有差异:美国的研究仅发现实际-应该自我差异与焦虑有关.本文对自我差异的文化学内容作了尝试性分析.实验Ⅱ考察“自我差异作为认知结构存在”的可能性及其与情绪的关系的性质.实际-理想自我差异较高、较低两组各12名被试对控制类、焦虑类形容词各12个作“是否适合描述自我”的判断,随后对判断过的形容词作自由回忆.结果表明:(1)两组被试自由回忆量无显着差异;(2)高自我差异组对焦虑类形容词反应时较短.该结果支持了实验假设:个体自我差异可能构成独特的认知结构,与特定情绪相联结,提高对该情绪信息的加工效率,对情绪有启动效应.  相似文献   

3.
为探讨大学生早期适应不良图式、焦虑与拖延的关系, 并考察焦虑在早期适应不良图式与拖延之间的中介效应, 采用一般拖延量表、Young图式问卷—简版、状态—特质焦虑问卷和自编一般人口学情况调查表, 对广东一高校559名大学生进行调查研究。研究结果发现:(1)"分离和拒绝"、"自主性和能力不足"、"缺乏自控/自律不足"图式均能显著正向预测焦虑和拖延;(2)特质焦虑能显著正向预测拖延, 而状态焦虑对拖延无显著预测作用;(3)特质焦虑在"分离和拒绝"、"自主性和能力不足"、"缺乏自控/自律不足"图式与拖延之间起部分中介作用。  相似文献   

4.
以往研究表明对死亡不可避免性的认识使人类产生死亡焦虑,并且为了降低该焦虑,年轻人倾向于抑制生理自我加工;然而相比年轻人,老年人对死亡的接受程度更高,并且自我抑制能力减弱。因此本研究采用死亡启动范式,探讨了死亡对自我相关记忆的影响以及可能存在的年龄差异。实验一以大学生为被试,结果发现死亡启动后,个体对自我相关信息的再认率显著低于消极启动后;实验二以老年人为被试,结果发现死亡启动后,个体对自我相关信息的再认率与消极启动后无差异。上述结果说明,死亡启动对自我相关记忆的影响存在年龄差异。在大学生中,死亡启动使个体抑制自我相关信息,而在老年人中该抑制效应并不存在。  相似文献   

5.
初中生同伴群体交往与自我同一性发展研究   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
安秋玲 《心理科学》2008,31(6):1524-1526,1498
使用社会认知地图(SCM)与自我同一性状态客观测量的标准化同卷(EOM-EIS-II),对初中生群体交往与自我同-性发展的关系进行研究,结果发现:(1)初中生普遍存在群体归属需要,绝大多数人都认为自己从属于一个或多个群体.(2)群体类型不影响自我同-性发展,班级内不同群体类型间,个体的自我同-性状态不存在显著差异.(3)群体内的个体地位影响自我同-性发展,处于群体中不同地位的个体.其自我同-性状态存在显著差异.  相似文献   

6.
为探讨社交焦虑与网络游戏过度使用的关系,并着力考察自我补偿动机和外倾性在其中所起的作用,对689名大学生进行调查,结果发现:(1)社交焦虑显著正向预测网络游戏过度使用;自我补偿动机在两者间起中介作用;(2)外倾性在社交焦虑通过自我补偿动机影响网络游戏过度使用的中介模型前半段起调节作用,对于高外倾性的大学生,社交焦虑对自我补偿动机有正向预测作用;对于低外倾性的大学生,社交焦虑对自我补偿动机影响不显著。  相似文献   

7.
田芊  邓士昌  郭佳 《心理科学》2012,35(5):1096-1101
我国约有一半的大学生经受着考试焦虑的困扰。结合国内外已有研究成果,本研究以624名大学生为被试,考察了自我决定动机的3种类型和拖延行为的2种类型对考试焦虑的忧虑性、情绪性的影响。结果发现:(1) 无动机对考试焦虑的2种类型都有正向的直接影响。(2) 唤起性拖延仅对情绪性有正向直接影响,而回避性拖延仅对忧虑性有正向直接影响。(3) 唤起性拖延在无动机影响考试焦虑的情绪性中起了重要的部分中介作用,而回避性拖延在无动机影响考试焦虑的忧虑性中起了重要的部分中介作用。  相似文献   

8.
大学生外语焦虑、自我效能感与外语成绩关系的研究   总被引:52,自引:0,他引:52       下载免费PDF全文
以315名大学生为研究对象,采用t检验、相关分析、方差分析和回归分析技术探讨了不同成绩水平、不同性别、不同专业学生的外语焦虑、自我效能感和外语成绩的差异及三者之间的关系。结果表明:(1)外语成绩及格组学生的外语焦虑水平显著低于不及格组学生,自我效能感显著高于不及格组学生;女大学生的外语成绩、自我效能感及能力因子和挫折因子上的自我效能感显著高于男大学生,男女大学生的外语焦虑水平无显著性差异;文科大学生的外语焦虑水平和外语成绩显著高于理科大学生,文、理科学生的自我效能感无显著性差异;(2)外语焦虑与外语成绩呈显著负相关,与自我效能感呈显著负相关,自我效能感与外语成绩呈显著正相关,性别、专业、外语焦虑、自我效能感、效能感的能力和挫折两个因子是外语成绩的显著预测变量。  相似文献   

9.
目的:探讨大学生的自我同一性发展状态、主观幸福感和核心自我评价之间的关系。方法:采用自行翻译的核心自我评价量表,生活满意度量表和情感平衡量表对江苏省南通市310名大学生进行调查。结果:(1)方差分析表明,核心自我评价在延缓状态和获得状态下均差异显著;延缓和获得状态在主观幸福感的三维度上均差异显著;(2)相关分析表明,延缓状态和获得状态下的核心自我评价与生活满意度、正性和负性情感的相关显著;(3)回归分析表明,自我同一性对核心自我评价和主观幸福感各维度回归效应显著,核心自我评价在自我同一性与主观幸福感之间存在部分中介效应。结论:自我同一性和核心自我评价均可作为主观幸福感的预测变量,核心自我评价是自我同一性和主观幸福感之间的一个中介变量。  相似文献   

10.
目的:调查并探讨大学生自我同一性状态发展特点。方法:从湖南某大学随机抽取320名大学生,采用王树青等人修订的EOM-EIS-II量表作为测量青少年自我同一性的工具。结果:(1)大学生自我同一性的性别、独生和非独生、城乡的差异比较结果显示,不同性别、独生非独生大学生在自我同一性的一些维度上存在显著差异,而来自城市和来自农村的大学生之间不存在差异;(2)不同年级大学生的同一性状态得分的方差分析结果显示,不同年级大学生在总体获得、总体延缓、总体早闭、意识早闭、人际早闭、人际延缓和人际获得7个维度上差异显著。结论:大学生自我同一性的发展存在一定的不平衡性。  相似文献   

11.
大学生特质焦虑:结构及其特点   总被引:10,自引:0,他引:10       下载免费PDF全文
本研究采用自编大学生特质焦虑问卷和斯皮尔伯格特质焦虑问卷测查了497名1至4年级大学生的特质焦虑.结果表明:①自编特质焦虑问卷可以提取五个因子,分别命名为学习焦虑,就业焦虑,人际焦虑,健康焦虑和惧怕否定评价焦虑;②自编问卷有较好的信度和效度,其克伦巴赫a系数、各维度与总分的相关以及与Spielberger焦虑问卷的相关均达到了心理测量学所认可的标准;③在学习焦虑上,有显著的年级差异,年级越高焦虑水平越低;在就业焦虑上,有显著的城乡差异和性别差异,农村学生的焦虑水平高于城市学生,女生高于男生.  相似文献   

12.
Abstract

Research has indicated that women may be particularly prone to experiencing mathematics anxiety and, to account for this, the sex-role socialization hypothesis posits that women are more math anxious because they have had less experience with mathematics and have more negative opinions regarding mathematics. In contrast, the math experiences hypothesis suggests that math anxiety is not truly a gender-related phenomenon, but rather is due to poor mathematical preparation, regardless of gender. The present study pitted the “sex-role” hypothesis against the “math experience” hypothesis in an examination of the relation between gender and math anxiety. One hundred and seventy-one university students completed a measure of math anxiety and a series of questions regarding their math marks, opinions, and experiences throughout their educational history. The findings clearly supported the math experience hypothesis of math anxiety, for differences in math marks, opinions, and experiences were found between non-math-anxious, moderately math-anxious, and highly math-anxious students. The few gender differences that were found indicated that the female students frequently reported higher math marks than those reported by male students. We suggest that the relation between gender and mathematics anxiety may be accounted for by gender-linked reporting biases. The limitations of the study are addressed and suggestions for future research are made.  相似文献   

13.
This study addressed the question if socially anxious adolescents have a negatively biased perception of the way they are treated by their peers. A total of 998 high school students from Grades 8-10 were categorized as socially low, middle, or high anxious on the basis of their SAS-A score. The perceived behavior of classmates was measured using three lists that described class behaviors during oral presentations of students, one list was concerned with the behaviors directed towards the student him/herself and the other two with behaviors directed towards a hypothetical high and low socially anxious peer, respectively. The results indicated that high socially anxious students felt negatively treated by their peers and that the other students too perceived that socially anxious classmates were treated more negatively. This suggests that the perception of the high socially anxious students is not distorted but based on the actual treatment they receive from their classmates.  相似文献   

14.
‘Jumping-to-Conclusions’ (JTC) is a data-gathering bias characterised by hasty decision-making, and is typically seen in individuals with high levels of delusions or paranoia. JTC has also been found in people with high trait and state anxiety. The present study aimed to explore the relationship between JTC and trait social anxiety and state anxiety, given paranoia is common in both social anxiety and psychotic disorders. One-hundred-and-eighty-six undergraduate students were allocated to a manipulation or control condition, and classified as high or low socially anxious. All participants completed the ‘beads task’ to assess JTC, and the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (state subscale) to assess state anxiety. Participants in the manipulation condition were given an anxiety-inducing situation. Although the manipulation was effective in inducing state anxiety, there was no significant correlation between JTC and trait or state social anxiety. High socially anxious individuals showed more conservative decision-making than controls over time, which was posited to be caused by inhibited working memory resulting from increased state anxiety.  相似文献   

15.
Anxiety in elderly persons is frequently discussed but rarely measured. Difficulties in measuring state and trait anxiety with mental health clients aged 55 and older using the Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) led the authors to validate the use of the simpler, children's form of their scale. Four types of validity data were found: (1) the childrens' form was highly correlated with the adult form, (2) both scales were significantly correlated with objective ratings of anxiety made by staff, (3) the STAI measures were not correlated with behavioral ratings other than anxiety (discriminant validity), (4) the residential clients were significantly higher in A-trait than the day clients. The nonresidential clients were not more anxious than college students. Discussion emphasizes the necessity of distinguishing between and measuring both state and trait anxiety in order to understand anxiety in the elderly.  相似文献   

16.
Two studies aimed to examine whether high socially anxious individuals are more likely to negatively interpret ambiguous social scenarios and facial expressions compared to low socially anxious individuals. We also examined whether interpretation bias serves as a mediator of the relationship between trait social anxiety and state anxiety responses, in particular current state anxiety, bodily sensations, and perceived probability and cost of negative evaluation pertaining to a speech task. Study 1 used ambiguous social scenarios and Study 2 used ambiguous facial expressions as stimuli to objectively assess interpretation bias. Undergraduate students with high and low social anxiety completed measures of state anxiety responses at three time points: baseline, after the interpretation bias task, and after the preparation for an impromptu speech. Results showed that high socially anxious individuals were more likely to endorse threat interpretations for ambiguous social scenarios and to interpret ambiguous faces as negative than low socially anxious individuals. Furthermore, negative interpretations mediated the relationship between trait social anxiety and perceived probability of negative evaluation pertaining to the speech task in Study 1 but not Study 2. The present studies provide new insight into the role of interpretation bias in social anxiety.  相似文献   

17.
刘静远  李虹 《心理学报》2019,51(7):747-758
探讨状态焦虑对时距知觉的影响以及注意偏向与认知评价在其中的中介和调节作用。招募大学生60人, 随机分为高、低状态焦虑组, 用情绪诱导程序诱导高、低状态焦虑; 用点探测范式测量注意偏向; 用时间再现任务测量时距知觉; 用视觉模拟心境量表测量认知评价。结果发现:(1)状态焦虑会导致对2000 ms时距的高估; (2)注意偏向在状态焦虑对2000 ms时距知觉的影响中具有部分中介作用; (3)状态焦虑通过注意偏向影响时距知觉的中介过程受到认知评价的调节作用:只有当认知评价得分较高时, 即个体认为焦虑对心理健康有害程度较高时, 状态焦虑通过注意偏向影响2000 ms时距知觉。研究结果揭示了焦虑个体时距知觉的内部过程, 丰富了焦虑通过注意偏向影响时距知觉的解释视角, 为通过认知评价和注意偏向的调整改善焦虑个体时距偏差现象提供了重要参考。  相似文献   

18.
Kendzierski and Sheffield (2000) found that exerciser schematics made less stable attributions for an exercise lapse than did aschematics; this could occur because they either perceived similar causes differently or encountered less stable obstacles to exercise. This research tested the perceptual explanation by examining the stability of the attributions that undergraduates who did versus did not have an exerciser self-schema made for specified lapses that they imagined as having happened to themselves versus another student. It also explored whether exerciser self-schema status was associated with differences in attributions of personal and external control. Consistent with the perceptual explanation, students with an exerciser self-schema made less stable attributions for a lapse imagined as having happened to themselves than did students without an exerciser self-schema, but equally stable attributions for a lapse imagined as having happened to another. Moreover, a content analysis revealed that the 2 groups cited similar causes for their own imagined lapses, providing further evidence that the difference in perceived stability was due to the groups perceiving similar causes differently. Exerciser self-schema status was not associated with attributions of either personal control or (in any clear way) external control.  相似文献   

19.
Joiner TE  Walker RL 《心理评价》2002,14(4):462-466
The properties of the main measure of acculturative stress have not been systematically examined. The authors conducted a study of the construct validity of the Social, Attitudinal, Familial, and Environmental Acculturative Stress Scale (A. M. Padilla, Y. Wagatsuma, & K. J. Lindholm, 1985) in a sample of 248 African American university students (156 of whom attended a historically Black college). After controlling for general life stress, the authors found that the relations of acculturative stress to depressive and anxious (but not suicidal) symptoms remained. Also, African American students at a historically Black college reported less acculturative stress than African American students at a large state university. These findings clarify the nature of a key construct and measure of acculturative stress.  相似文献   

20.
The aim of this study is to examine the determinants of fruit and vegetable (F&V) consumption with the application of the Theory of Planned Behaviour (TPB) and the additional variables of self-schema, habit and self-regulation. While it has been shown that those with a healthy-eater self-schema are more likely to carry out their healthy dietary intentions, the underlying processes that influence this relationship have received limited empirical attention. Recent findings on dietary behaviour suggest that self-regulatory ability and habit strength may have dissimilar effects on the intention-behaviour relationship within schematics and non-schematics. Self-report questionnaires regarding F&V consumption cognitions and two tests of self-regulation were administered to 209 university students. One week later, participants completed questionnaires on their behaviour. The TPB significantly predicted intentions and prospective behaviour. Self-schema did not moderate the relationship between intention and behaviour. However, within healthy-eater schematics, those with high intention and high self-regulatory ability were more likely to consume F&V, while within non-schematics, those with low intention and high habit strength were more likely to consume F&V. The findings support the use of the TPB in predicting F&V consumption and the validity of the self-schema distinction. Implications for designing interventions are discussed.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号