首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到18条相似文献,搜索用时 156 毫秒
1.
采用E-prime编排认知实验研究照明环境与TFT-LCD视觉显示终端界面背景饱和度的交互效应对视觉绩效的影响。依据字母视力表设计原理制作认知材料,以认知速度与准确率为视觉绩效评价指标。结果发现,照度与饱和度的交互效应对视觉绩效存在显著影响,色温与饱和度的交互效应仅对准确率存在显著影响,三因素交互作用对准确率存在显著影响。结果表明,不同照度、色温下,通过调节显示终端界面背景饱和度可改善视觉绩效。  相似文献   

2.
本研究采用单因素被试间设计,考察了白天室内照度(300 lx vs 1200 lx,眼位水平)对认知加工的影响及主观情绪和警觉性在照度水平与认知加工间的中介效应。结果发现:(1)被试在高照度照明条件下的认知加工绩效(Go/No-go,2-Back)明显好于低照度照明;(2)高照度照明对积极情绪和主观警觉性具有显著的正向影响;(3)警觉性在照度水平对Go/No-go任务的影响中起着完全中介作用,情绪的中介作用不显著。这表明警觉性在室内照度与部分认知加工间扮演着重要的"桥梁"作用。  相似文献   

3.
照度和对比度对于视觉显示终端视觉功能的影响   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
本研究试图通过视觉显示终端的视觉功能研究,了解图象在荧光屏上的亮度对比度、荧光屏照度以及不同光源对于视觉功能的作用。图象在荧光屏上的亮度对比是影响阅读速度的重要因素,同时,荧光屏照度增加时,阅读速度降低。为了提高工作效率,可以提高图象在荧光屏上的亮度对比,也可以降低荧光屏照度。我们似乎可以推荐合适的荧光屏照度为50—200Lux。这时水平工作面照度是70—340Lux。  相似文献   

4.
已有研究表明环境照度、计算机显示器的屏幕亮度和对比度会影响用户的任务操作绩效。本研究采用视觉搜索任务,从行为绩效、主观评价以及眼睛疲劳度指标来考察环境照度、手机屏幕亮度对视觉搜索绩效的影响,并提出在不同的环境照度下手机屏幕亮度的最优值设置参数。结果表明:(1)当手机屏幕亮度保持不变时,不同环境亮度下的搜索绩效差异显著。(2)当环境照度为0lx时,手机屏幕亮度的最优值在11cd/m2左右;环境照度为100lx时,手机屏幕亮度的最优值是68cd/m2左右,环境照度为500lx时,手机屏幕亮度最优值是257cd/m2左右。  相似文献   

5.
VDT屏面亮度和文件背景照度对视觉核读作业的影响   总被引:6,自引:0,他引:6  
研究VDT屏面亮度和文件背景照度对视觉核读作业的影响效应。以核读速度、限肌调节辐合时间和主观舒适性为测评指标。结果上述三项指标均随VDT屏面亮度和文件背景照度的变化而发生变化。结论推荐VDT屏面亮度的合适范围为15~20cd/m^2,文件背景照度的合适范围为300~450lux。VDT屏而亮度与背景照度水平的匹配性也应加以考虑。  相似文献   

6.
颜色突显是指在与视觉搜索任务有关的视觉显示中,采用颜色突出显示多个项目中的目标项目,从而提高视觉搜索效率的一种方法或技术。本文采用视觉搜索任务,以正确率和反应时为指标对单彩色和复杂彩色背景下不同颜色突显的视觉搜索绩效进行了研究。结果表明:(1)在单彩色背景和复杂彩色背景下,颜色突显条件视觉搜索绩效显著优于非突显条件;(2)在浅蓝或浅绿的单彩色背景下,四种颜色突显绩效中,红色突显绩效最好,紫色最差,而在随机彩色背景和复杂彩色背景下,颜色突显绩效差异并不显著;(3)在彩色背景下的颜色突显绩效会受到干扰数字的颜色的影响,其中在单色和随机彩色背景下,干扰目标颜色为黑色时的视觉搜索绩效优于干扰目标颜色为白色时的情景,而在自然彩色背景下,这种视觉搜索优势并不存在。  相似文献   

7.
以往个体和团队冲突研究主要考察了个人特征或团队特征对冲突结果的影响, 而尚未充分关注冲突管理过程中个体特征与团队特征间的交互效应。基于个人-团队匹配理论, 本研究探讨了个体层面宜人性与团队层面宜人性异质性对团队中个体冲突(关系冲突、任务冲突)和工作绩效间关系的影响。基于来自64个银行服务团队(包含339名下属和64名主管)的多来源、多时点纵向数据, 本研究所得结果显示:(1)关系冲突显著负向影响工作绩效, 任务冲突对工作绩效的影响不显著。(2)个体宜人性能够显著减弱关系冲突对工作绩效的负面影响, 而增强任务冲突对工作绩效的正向影响。(3)关系/任务冲突、个体宜人性和团队宜人性异质性间存在着三重交互效应, 共同影响工作绩效。具体而言, 当团队宜人性异质性水平较低时, 个体宜人性对关系/任务冲突与工作绩效间关系的调节作用更加显著。  相似文献   

8.
本研究使用空间任务-转换范式,控制视、听刺激的突显性,探讨自下而上注意对视觉主导效应的影响。结果表明视、听刺激突显性显著地影响视觉主导效应,实验一中当听觉刺激为高突显性时,视觉主导效应显著减弱。实验二中当听觉刺激为高突显性并且视觉刺激为低突显性时,视觉主导效应进一步减弱但依然存在。结果支持偏向竞争理论,在跨通道视听交互过程中视觉刺激更具突显性,在多感觉整合过程中更具加工优势。  相似文献   

9.
朱海腾 《心理科学》2019,(5):1217-1223
我军广泛实行分工负责军事和政治工作的平级双领导体制,两名主官的相互作用可能产生额外影响效果。本研究在“连”这一双领导军事团队中,构建了一个以士气为中介的交互效应模型,考察连长和指导员的魅力型领导对连队绩效的影响。对350名军校士兵学员进行问卷调查,结果显示:(1)连长和指导员的魅力型领导对连队绩效具有交互效应。(2)士气在连长、指导员的魅力型领导与连队绩效之间均具有中介效应。(3)连长、指导员的魅力型领导对连队绩效的交互效应受到士气的中介。(4)双主官的交互效应为补偿式:与低魅力型领导搭档时,高魅力型领导的积极效果凸显,但两位高魅力型领导的协同效应不显著。  相似文献   

10.
朱海腾 《心理科学》2005,(5):1217-1223
我军广泛实行分工负责军事和政治工作的平级双领导体制,两名主官的相互作用可能产生额外影响效果。本研究在“连”这一双领导军事团队中,构建了一个以士气为中介的交互效应模型,考察连长和指导员的魅力型领导对连队绩效的影响。对350名军校士兵学员进行问卷调查,结果显示:(1)连长和指导员的魅力型领导对连队绩效具有交互效应。(2)士气在连长、指导员的魅力型领导与连队绩效之间均具有中介效应。(3)连长、指导员的魅力型领导对连队绩效的交互效应受到士气的中介。(4)双主官的交互效应为补偿式:与低魅力型领导搭档时,高魅力型领导的积极效果凸显,但两位高魅力型领导的协同效应不显著。  相似文献   

11.
This study constructed the image quality models for a small mobile display under different ambient illumination levels using Group Method and Data Handling (GMDH) and described the relationship between perceived image quality and physical measurements. 33 college students took part in this experiment and were asked to evaluate the image quality under 1500 lux (typical indoor office illumination) and 7000 lux (simulated outdoor environment) in Stage One and Stage Two, respectively. In each stage, the participants had to evaluate 21 images. 17 sets of the data as a training set were used to build the model and four sets of the data as a testing set were used to verify the model. The results indicated that the effects of luminance, contrast, correlated color temperature (CCT), and resolution were significant on perceived image quality under 1500 lux. However, color temperature was not a significant physical characteristic, and an interaction between luminance and contrast was found below 7000 lux. From the results of the experiment, it is considered that the outdoor environment (7000 lux) is not suitable for using mobile displays. Finally, once a valid image quality model is built, the subjective image quality can be established when the measurements of significant physical characteristics are provided. The results of subjective ratings can also be provided for mobile display manufacturers to improve the product quality so that their products can meet customers' requirements.  相似文献   

12.
The effect of retinal locus on the letter-span error function was investigated by requiring Ss to fixate at the left, middle, or right of lines of letters presented for 100 msec. Stimulus presentations consisted of lines of eight letters presented across the visual field, and lines of four letters presented alternately in the left and right visual hemifields. Locus of fixation and relative letter position were found to be significantly related to whether or not a letter was correctly localized. A significant interaction between locus of fixation and letter position was observed.  相似文献   

13.
孙崇勇  刘电芝 《心理科学》2016,39(4):869-874
本研究采用2×2的多因素混合实验设计,探讨了物理学习环境之一的学习材料背景颜色对认知负荷及学习的影响。结果发现:(1)背景颜色在对认知负荷的影响上与任务类型产生交互作用,在解决创造性问题时,暖色调(如红色)背景下的认知负荷显著高于冷色调(如蓝色)。(2)从学习成绩上看,暖色调(如红色)更有利于记忆测试问题的解决,冷色调(如蓝色)更有利于创造性问题的解决。(3)认知负荷与任务绩效呈一定程度相关。  相似文献   

14.
为了探究视觉负后像对视觉搜索效率是否会通过颜色这一特征产生干扰,结合Van Lier后像和视觉搜索范式,通过两个实验考察了负后像对视觉搜索效率的干扰效应,并对负后像填充区域和原始发生区域进行了对比。实验结果表明,在有负后像的情况下,视觉搜索效率明显降低,但是不同颜色之间的差异并不显著;而负后像填充区域的干扰效应要弱于原始发生区域。研究结论对于进一步提高视觉搜索效率的颜色设计以及视觉负后像的新研究方法具有重要的参考意义。  相似文献   

15.
A novel brightness illusion in planar patterns is reported. The illusion occurs, for example, when surfaces with a luminance ramp shaded from black to white are positioned on a black homogeneous background, so that each white end of the surfaces faces a single point of the plane of the pattern. The illusion consists of the enhancement of the brightness of the background in a relatively wide area around the white ends of the surfaces. A parametric study was conducted in which participants were asked to rate the difference in brightness between the parts of the background inside and outside a virtual circle formed by disks with different luminance ramps. The results show that mean ratings of brightness depended on the luminance of the background, the luminance range of ramps, and the kind of ramp. Discussion of these results with reference to other brightness illusions (assimilation, neon color spreading, anomalous surfaces, visual phantoms, grating induction, and the glare effect) shows that t hephantom illumination illusion derives from processes producing the perception of ambient illumination.  相似文献   

16.
On-line evidence of visual constraints in reading is reported for measures other than eye behaviour. Text was read from a CRT using a moving window between 6 and 19 characters wide, with letters outside the window replaced by blanks, dashes, or visually similar letters. Reading speed was self-adjusted; subjects performed a subsidiary task while reading aloud. Window width and the visual context beyond the window affected dual task performance, and together with fount size, also reading accuracy. The form of text background further affected two measures of eye-voice span. These findings complement ones for ocular behaviour obtained using a CRT window coordinated with a reader's eye fixations.  相似文献   

17.
A visual search task was used to assess developmental changes in children's selective attention to specified portions of a visual display. Seven-, nine-, and twelve-year-olds searched for a target letter in matrices of letters, each of which was centered in a form. On each matrix the forms were uniform or they varied in color, shape or both color and shape. The children searched with either no cues or with color or shape cues that could be used to restrict and speed their search. In all conditions search speed increased with age. Comparisons among conditions revealed three different age trends. With no cues children of all ages were slowed comparably by variation in background forms. With color cues all children increased their search speeds relative to no-cue speeds, suggesting selective fixation, but the 12-year-olds benefited most from the cues. With shape cues the search speed of 9- and 12-year-olds was slowed while that of 7-year-olds was either unchanged or was slowed only slightly. These different trends caution against overly general statements of changes with age in selective attention, and highlight the need to consider both particular task requirements and the processes used by subjects of different ages in tasks requiring selective attention.  相似文献   

18.
The primate visual system responds to shapes, colors, and various other features more strongly in some brain areas than others. However, it remains unclear how these features are bound together so that an object with all its attributes is perceived. A patient (R.M.) with bilateral parietal-occipital lesions has been shown previously to miscombine the shape and color of items, making errors known as illusory conjunctions (ICs). In this study, we examined the effects of a third feature (motion) on this patient's IC rates. R.M. was presented with two letters that moved in different ways. He often reported seeing the shape of one of the letters with the other letter's motion. His performance on the same task with three features shows that correctly combining two features did not necessarily lead to correctly binding the third. These data support modularity of feature representations in the human brain and provide supporting evidence that spatial representations associated with the parietal lobe are necessary for normal feature integration.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号