首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 140 毫秒
1.
孙青青  陈本友  赵伶俐 《心理科学》2011,34(6):1332-1337
颜色偏好是人在心理上所喜爱或偏好的颜色。人们对抽象颜色、具体物色都会表现出偏好;性格、性别、年龄、文化等因素会影响颜色偏好。以往研究者在研究内容、研究材料、研究条件尚存不足,这正是今后研究的方向。  相似文献   

2.
伴随“晚婚与不婚”“低生育率危机”的热议,女性择偶偏好问题受到关注。本研究采用问卷法和实验法,考察父亲在位对女儿择偶偏好的影响及母亲态度的调节作用。结果:(1)父亲在位会影响女儿择偶偏好,父亲在位水平越高,女儿越偏好与其父亲长相相似个体;(2)母亲态度在父亲在位对女儿择偶偏好影响中起调节作用。母亲态度越积极,父亲在位对女儿择偶偏好影响的正向预测作用越显著。该研究对指导女性择偶偏好、提高婚育率有重要意义。  相似文献   

3.
目的:了解住院患儿颜色偏好特点,为患儿创建适合心理特点的住院休养环境提供依据。方法自制2组颜色卡片,采用对偶比较法对30名住院患儿进行了7种颜色的偏好研究。结果:1.30名住院患儿颜色偏好结果依次为:黄,蓝,橙,粉,绿,红,白。2.有无白血病患儿颜色偏好顺序不同,红色与白色为两类患儿最不喜爱的颜色。3.不同性别患儿的颜色偏好顺序不同。结论了解住院患儿颜色偏好的特点对于充实相关理论,改善住院环境、提升护理质量、促进病人康复都具有一定的意义。  相似文献   

4.
抽象颜色偏好实验研究   总被引:6,自引:0,他引:6  
本实验采用对偶比较法对4~21岁人群779名被试进行抽象颜色偏好调查研究,得出以下结果:(1)4~21岁人群抽象颜色偏好顺序依次是蓝、橙、红、黄、紫、绿;(2)男生和女生对抽象颜色的偏好具有明最差异;(3)不同年龄段的人群时抽象颜色偏好具有明显差异;(4)抽象颜色偏好虽然随着时代的变迁发生变化,但是仍有一定的规律性。  相似文献   

5.
徐展  闫丹 《心理科学》2015,(2):496-499
颜色偏好是人在心理上所喜爱或偏好的颜色,性别是影响颜色偏好的重要因素之一。本文区分一般人群和特殊人群,结合不同的心理发展阶段,评述对抽象颜色和具体物色的偏好的性别差异研究进展,具体比较了打猎者-采摘者理论、视锥-对立对比成分理论、性别图式理论以及生态效价理论等,并且讨论分析了以往研究者在内容和理论解释等方面存在的不足。未来研究者应采用多元化的方法,深入探索颜色偏好性别差异的机制,期望整合得到解释力更强的理论观点。  相似文献   

6.
4~6岁幼儿颜色偏好实验研究   总被引:4,自引:1,他引:3  
刘少英  葛列众  朱瑶 《心理科学》2004,27(3):669-670
本实验采用对偶比较法.对4—6岁幼儿进行颜色偏好实验研究,结果表明:在4—6岁的年龄段中,幼儿颜色偏好顺序依次为:红、黄、橙、蓝、紫、绿;男女幼儿的颜色偏好有明显差异;不同年龄之间的幼儿颜色偏好没有明显差异。  相似文献   

7.
已有研究表明典型群体成员会表现出更强的群际偏差。然而,典型群体成员表现群际偏差的基础是内群体偏好还是外群体贬损尚未明确。本研究以中国社会背景下城市居民与农民工的群际关系为例,对典型城市居民表现群际偏差的基础进行探索。研究一采用相关研究设计考察典型性与群际偏差的关系,结果表明越典型的城市居民会表现越强的内群体偏好,但外群体贬损不受典型性影响。研究二采用实验设计对城市居民典型性进行操纵,结果进一步证实了典型性会导致内群体偏好,而非外群体贬损。研究结果一致地表明,典型城市居民表现群际偏差的基础是内群体偏好而非外群体贬损。  相似文献   

8.
张佩  夏勉 《心理科学进展》2015,23(6):1009-1020
服药依从性是影响抑郁症患者疗效和复发的重要因素, 然而很多研究发现抑郁症患者的服药依从性较低。与服药依从性有关的概念有服从性、依从性和遵从性。抑郁症服药依从性的影响因素包括疾病与治疗因素、医生相关因素、患者相关因素及文化与环境因素四个方面, 主要从医生和患者两方面进行干预。其评定方法包括主观和客观两种。介绍了研究趋势, 未来应发展服药依从性测量工具, 加强本土化和社会文化因素的研究, 加强共病对抑郁症服药依从性影响的研究, 制定个别化的干预措施, 并加强心理学在抑郁症服药依从性领域的交叉研究。  相似文献   

9.
药物口服是临床常用的给药途径。口服药的服用时间有餐前、餐时、餐后、睡前等几种类型。药物的服药时间是根据每种药物的药效学、药动学的不同及药物与食物的关系等多种因素决定的。根据人体生物节律与时展药理学的关系,参考说明书和药学参考书,总结了门诊常用口服药的服用时间,并指出了各种服药时间的优缺点。  相似文献   

10.
通过采用眼动追踪和主观偏好评定实验分别考察以不同对比度极性、颜色组合呈现的电子线性文本和引导式文本对阅读疲劳的影响。实验1采用电子线性文本形式,考察不同对比度极性和颜色组合对视觉疲劳的影响以及主观偏好和主观疲劳感。结果表明:在电子线性文本中,黄底黑字、绿底黑字和黑底绿字的颜色组合引起的视觉疲劳显著少于其它组合;不同对比度极性的视觉疲劳度无显著差异,并且读者更偏好正极性文本。实验2采用引导式文本形式考察不同对比度极性和颜色组合对视觉疲劳的影响以及主观偏好和主观疲劳感。结果表明:在引导式文本中,不同颜色组合的视觉疲劳无显著差异;而正极性文本引起的视觉疲劳显著高于负极性文本,同样读者更显著偏好正极性文本。结论是读者在电子线性文本和引导式文本的视觉疲劳度存在差异,对阅读疲劳度,电子线性文本反应在不同颜色组合上,而引导式文本则在对比度极性上,而读者对电子文本和引导式文本的主观偏好度一致,即更偏好正极性文本。  相似文献   

11.
We explored infants’ ability to recognize the canonical colors of daily objects, including two color-specific objects (human face and fruit) and a non-color-specific object (flower), by using a preferential looking technique. A total of 58 infants between 5 and 8 months of age were tested with a stimulus composed of two color pictures of an object placed side by side: a correctly colored picture (e.g., red strawberry) and an inappropriately colored picture (e.g., green-blue strawberry). The results showed that, overall, the 6- to 8-month-olds showed preference for the correctly colored pictures for color-specific objects, whereas they did not show preference for the correctly colored pictures for the non-color-specific object. The 5-month-olds showed no significant preference for the correctly colored pictures for all object conditions. These findings imply that the recognition of canonical color for objects emerges at 6 months of age.  相似文献   

12.
Although models of visual search have often assumed that attention can only be set for a single feature or property at a time, recent studies have suggested that it may be possible to maintain more than one attentional control setting. The aim of the present study was to investigate whether spatial attention could be guided by multiple attentional control settings for color. In a standard spatial cueing task, participants searched for either of two colored targets accompanied by an irrelevantly colored distractor. Across five experiments, results consistently showed that nonpredictive cues matching either target color produced a significant spatial cueing effect, while irrelevantly colored cues did not. This was the case even when the target colors could not be linearly separated from irrelevantly cue colors in color space, suggesting that participants were not simply adopting one general color set that included both target colors. The results could not be explained by intertrial priming by previous targets, nor could they be explained by a single inhibitory set for the distractor color. Overall, the results are most consistent with the maintenance of multiple attentional control settings.  相似文献   

13.
We investigated cultural differences between U.S. and Japanese color preferences and the ecological factors that might influence them. Japanese and U.S. color preferences have both similarities (e.g., peaks around blue, troughs around dark‐yellow, and preferences for saturated colors) and differences (Japanese participants like darker colors less than U.S. participants do). Complex gender differences were also evident that did not conform to previously reported effects. Palmer and Schloss's (2010) weighted affective valence estimate (WAVE) procedure was used to test the Ecological Valence Theory's (EVT's) prediction that within‐culture WAVE‐preference correlations should be higher than between‐culture WAVE‐preference correlations. The results supported several, but not all, predictions. In the second experiment, we tested color preferences of Japanese–U.S. multicultural participants who could read and speak both Japanese and English. Multicultural color preferences were intermediate between U.S. and Japanese preferences, consistent with the hypothesis that culturally specific personal experiences during one's lifetime influence color preferences.  相似文献   

14.
李宏汀  王平飞 《心理科学》2012,35(4):848-851
前人研究表明:年龄影响颜色偏爱、抽象颜色偏爱不同于具体颜色偏爱。因此本研究在抽象颜色偏爱实验基础上通过设计高保真网页研究青年人、老年人的网页颜色偏爱(包括主题色及其与文本色的搭配)。结果表明:(1)年龄、性别不会影响网页颜色偏爱。(2)浅蓝色、蓝色比较适合作为网页主题色。(3)网页主题采用不同颜色时,被试对文本色偏爱表现出较高的规律性,黑色、蓝色更适合作为网页文本色。  相似文献   

15.
Bidirectional genetic selection of quail for early color preferences, for eighteen generations, resulted in nearly perfect choices of blue over red in one and red over blue in the other selected line. It also enhanced the preference of a grated over a dotted black-and-white pattern. Color and pattern preferences in hybrids of selected and control lines fell back to about halfway between parental values. Choices between composite stimuli of colors and patterns indicated summation of the respective, genetically influenced preference values, with partial dominance of color over pattern effects. Exposure to colors modified color choices. Exposure to colored patterns likewise modified color choices, but it did not change, or only marginally changed, choices between patterns. The phenotypic expression of this selective learning, however, was influenced by the genetically manipulated preference values and preference combinations of colors and patterns in the stimuli with which postexposure performances were tested. Overall, the data highlighted the need for concepts that would discriminate between the developmental and the episodic expression of genetic influences and genotype--environment interactions in behavior.  相似文献   

16.
赵荣  王小娟  杨剑峰 《心理学报》2016,48(8):915-923
探讨超音段(如声调)与音段信息的共同作用机制, 对口语词汇识别研究具有重要的理论意义。有研究探讨了声调在口语词汇语义通达阶段的作用, 但在相对早期的音节感知阶段, 声调与声母、韵母的共同作用机制还缺乏系统的认识。本研究采用oddball实验范式, 通过两个行为实验考察了声调在汉语音节感知中的作用。实验1发现检测声调和声母变化的时间没有差异, 均比检测韵母变化的时间长, 表明在汉语音节感知中对声调的敏感性不及韵母。实验2发现声母和韵母的组合并没有明显优于对韵母的觉察, 但声调与声母或韵母的同时变化都促进了被试对偏差刺激的觉察, 表明声调通过与声母、韵母的结合来共同影响汉语音节的感知加工。研究结果在认知行为层面为声调在音节感知中的作用机制提供了直接的实验证据, 为进一步探讨超音段与音段信息共同作用的认知神经机制提供了基础。  相似文献   

17.
In visual search tasks, observers can guide their attention towards items in the visual field that share features with the target item. In this series of studies, we examined the time course of guidance toward a subset of items that have the same color as the target item. Landolt Cs were placed on 16 colored disks. Fifteen distractor Cs had gaps facing up or down while one target C had a gap facing left or right. Observers searched for the target C and reported which side contained the gap as quickly as possible. In the absence of other information, observers must search at random through the Cs. However, during the trial, the disks changed colors. Twelve disks were now of one color and four disks were of another color. Observers knew that the target C would always be in the smaller color set. The experimental question was how quickly observers could guide their attention to the smaller color set. Results indicate that observers could not make instantaneous use of color information to guide the search, even when they knew which two colors would be appearing on every trial. In each study, it took participants 200–300 ms to fully utilize the color information once presented. Control studies replicated the finding with more saturated colors and with colored C stimuli (rather than Cs on colored disks). We conclude that segregation of a display by color for the purposes of guidance takes 200–300 ms to fully develop.  相似文献   

18.
When learning basic color vocabulary, young children show a selective delay in the acquisition of brown and gray relative to other basic color terms. In this study, we first establish the robustness of this finding and then investigate the extent to which perception, language, and color preference may influence color conceptualization. Experimental tasks were designed to measure different aspects of perceptual color processing (discrimination and saliency), color preference and objective counts of color term frequency in preschool-directed language (books and mothers' speech) were used to compare the acquisition of three groups of colors: primary colors, secondary colors (orange, pink, and purple) that appear at the same time as the primary colors, and secondary colors (brown and gray) that appear late. Although our results suggest that perception does not directly shape young children's color term acquisition, we found that children prefer brown and gray significantly less than basic colors and that these color terms appear significantly less often in child-directed speech, suggesting that color preference, linguistic input, and developing color cognition may be linked.  相似文献   

19.
采用启动Stroop范式, 对刻板印象激活的无意图性及其大脑神经活动特征进行了探讨。操纵了启动和“无启动”条件以及与启动所激活的刻板印象一致、冲突和无关的3类靶子词, 记录了36名大学生被试对靶子词进行颜色判别的行为反应和脑电图(EEG, electroencephalogram)。结果发现:(1)“无启动”条件下对3类靶子的反应时基本一致; 启动条件下对一致靶子做出颜色判别的反应时显著快于对冲突靶子的, 对无关靶子的反应时介于两者之间。(2)“无启动”条件下无关靶子诱发的N400波幅最大--显著大于一致和冲突靶子诱发的, 后两者诱发的N400无差异; 启动条件下一致靶子诱发的N400波幅显著小于冲突和无关靶子诱发的N400波幅, 无关靶子诱发的N400波幅也小于冲突靶子诱发的N400波幅。这表明刻板印象激活是一个发生在知觉后加工阶段的具有无意图性的自动化加工过程, 刻板印象激活效应具有抑制冲突信息加工同时促进一致信息加工的“双刃剑”模式, N400可以作为考察这一认知过程特征的电生理学指标。  相似文献   

20.
People prioritize those aspects of the visual environment that match their attentional set. In the present study, we investigated whether switching from one attentional set to another is associated with a cost. We asked observers to sequentially saccade toward two color-defined targets, one on the left side of the display, the other on the right, each among a set of heterogeneously colored distractors. The targets were of the same color (no attentional set switch required) or of different colors (switch of attentional sets necessary), with each color consistently tied to a side, to allow observers to maximally prepare for the switch. We found that saccades were less accurate and slower in the switch condition than in the no-switch condition. Furthermore, whenever one of the distractors had the color associated with the other attentional set, a substantial proportion of saccades did not end on the target, but on this distractor. A time course analysis revealed that this distractor preference turned into a target preference after about 250-300 ms, suggesting that this is the time required to switch attentional sets.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号