共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
Matthew J. Sharps Sandy S. Day Michael A. Nunes Amy Neff Ellen Woo 《Current Psychology》2004,22(4):379-393
Research indicates the presence of an age-related pictorial processing deficit, for which the elderly may attempt to compensate
through the use of relational information. Cognitive asynchrony theory, a recent synthetic formulation which unites elements
of the generalized slowing hypothesis, environmental support theory, and the item-specific/relational information distinction,
has proven useful in a number of experiments in explaining these aspects of visual cognitive aging. The present experiments
tested this theoretical formulation under high processing demand conditions in both the relational and the pictorial/item-specific
realm. Young and older adults yielded a complex pattern of results consistent with the cognitive asynchrony synthesis of these
theoretical considerations. The present experiments add to the growing body of findings indicating that the cognitive subsystems
of memory decline at different rates, that the differences in cognitive processing between young and older adults tend to
be more quantitative than qualitative, and that the global age-related memory deficits of popular belief are in fact relatively
circumscribed and specific.
This research was supported by grant AG11605 from the National Institute on Aging, and by a grant from the College of Science
and Mathematics, California State University. 相似文献
2.
Matthew J. Sharps Sandy S. Martin Michael A. Nunes Michelle Merrill 《Current Psychology》1999,18(3):254-271
Recent research indicates the presence of an age-related visual processing deficit, for which the elderly may attempt to compensate
through the use of relational information. This hypothesis was tested, using the category superiority effect as a model system.
In studies of young adults, the category superiority effect has been shown to be confined to relatively abstract stimulus
materials, such as verbal items, and to be absent for pictures. However, it was predicted that a category superiority effect
would be present in elderly adults both for verbal and for pictorial stimuli, since the elderly would be expected to utilize
category information to compensate for imagerie deficits. This prediction was confirmed, consistent with the hypothesis advanced
above. It was further suggested that the establishment of a prior framework for recall, based on relational information, would
reduce this effect significantly. This prediction was also confirmed.
This research was supported by Grant AGI 1605, National Institute on Aging, and by a grant from the School of Natural Sciences,
California State University, Fresno. 相似文献
3.
Matthew J. Sharps Sandy S. Martin Michael A. Nunes Michelle Merrill 《Current psychology (New Brunswick, N.J.)》1999,18(3):254-271
Recent research indicates the presence of an age-related visual processing deficit, for which the elderly may attempt to compensate
through the use of relational information. This hypothesis was tested, using the category superiority effect as a model system.
In studies of young adults, the category superiority effect has been shown to be confined to relatively abstract stimulus
materials, such as verbal items, and to be absent for pictures. However, it was predicted that a category superiority effect
would be present in elderly adults both for verbal and for pictorial stimuli, since the elderly would be expected to utilize
category information to compensate for imagerie deficits. This prediction was confirmed, consistent with the hypothesis advanced
above. It was further suggested that the establishment of a prior framework for recall, based on relational information, would
reduce this effect significantly. This prediction was also confirmed.
This research was supported by Grant AGI 1605, National Institute on Aging, and by a grant from the School of Natural Sciences,
California State University, Fresno. 相似文献
4.
Matthew J. Sharps Brandon T. Foster Sandy S. Martin Michael A. Nunes 《Current Psychology》1999,18(3):241-253
Previous studies have demonstrated that older adults can use visually distinctive cues to enhance their spatial memory performance
and their recall of the identities of items located in space. Other research has shown that older adults can also use relational
information to enhance recall. The relationship between relational and visuospatial support for memory across the adult lifespan
is complex and has been little explored. In the present study, this relationship was examined in tasks that measured free
recall for item sets possessing different degrees of visuospatial and relational support for recall. It was predicted that
both types of support would enhance older adult recall relative to that of the young, and that the two types of support would
synergize. The results were generally consistent with these predictions. These findings show that older adults can use relatively
simple relational or visuospatial support systems to enhance free recall significantly.
This research was supported by Grant AGI 1605, National Institute on Aging. 相似文献
5.
Matthew J. Sharps Brandon T. Foster Sandy S. Martin Michael A. Nunes 《Current psychology (New Brunswick, N.J.)》1999,18(3):241-253
Previous studies have demonstrated that older adults can use visually distinctive cues to enhance their spatial memory performance
and their recall of the identities of items located in space. Other research has shown that older adults can also use relational
information to enhance recall. The relationship between relational and visuospatial support for memory across the adult lifespan
is complex and has been little explored. In the present study, this relationship was examined in tasks that measured free
recall for item sets possessing different degrees of visuospatial and relational support for recall. It was predicted that
both types of support would enhance older adult recall relative to that of the young, and that the two types of support would
synergize. The results were generally consistent with these predictions. These findings show that older adults can use relatively
simple relational or visuospatial support systems to enhance free recall significantly.
This research was supported by Grant AGI 1605, National Institute on Aging. 相似文献
6.
Janet M. Duchek David A. Balota Mark E. Faust F. Richard Ferraro 《Neuropsychology, development, and cognition. Section B, Aging, neuropsychology and cognition》2013,20(2):156-167
Abstract The present study examines changes in healthy young and healthy older adults in the ability to inhibit partially activated information in a picture/word interference paradigm. On each trial, subjects received a cue (i.e., the word PICTURE or WORD) indicating which of two stimuli the subject should attend to in an upcoming picture/word display. the display always contained a superimposed picture and word (e.g., a picture of a DEER with the word TEA printed on it). Following display offset, and depending upon the initial precue, either a test picture (e.g., KETTLE) or a test word (e.g., MOOSE) was presented. the subject's task was to determine as quickly and as accurately as possible whether the test stimulus was related to the cued dimension of the earlier picture/word display. the speed to reject an unrelated test item (e.g., picture of a KETTLE when the precue was PICTURE) that was related to the ignored dimension of the picture/word display (e.g., the word TEA in the picture/word display) was used as an index of the efficiency of the inhibitory system. the results indicated that older adults had more difficulty than younger adults inhibiting a to-be-ignored word when it was related to a picture test item, but did not have more difficulty inhibiting a to-be-ignored picture when it was related to a word test item. the results indicate that an age-related deficit in the control of interfering information is dependent upon the fluency of the processing routes. 相似文献
7.
Reasoning requires making inferences based on information gleaned from a set of relations. The relational complexity of a problem increases with the number of relations that must be considered simultaneously to make a correct inference. Previous work (Viskontas, Morrison, Holyoak, Hummel, & Knowlton, ) has shown that older adults have difficulty integrating multiple relations during analogical reasoning, especially when required to inhibit irrelevant information. We report two experiments that examined the ability to integrate multiple relations in younger, middle-aged, and older adults performing two other reasoning tasks. These tasks systematically varied relational complexity, and required either inductive reasoning (a version of the Raven's Matrices Task) or transitive inference. Our results show that as people age they have increasing difficulty in solving problems that require them to integrate multiple relations. This difficulty may stem from a decrease in working memory capacity. 相似文献
8.
Rebekah E. Smith R. Reed Hunt 《Neuropsychology, development, and cognition. Section B, Aging, neuropsychology and cognition》2014,21(4):411-431
Remembering to perform an action in the future, called prospective memory, often shows age-related differences in favor of young adults when tested in the laboratory. Recently Smith, Horn, and Bayen (2012; Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 19, 495) embedded a PM task in an ongoing color-matching task and manipulated the difficulty of the ongoing task by varying the number of colors on each trial of the task. Smith et al. found that age-related differences in PM performance (lower PM performance for older adults relative to young adults) persisted even when older adults could perform the ongoing task as well or better than the young adults. The current study investigates a possible explanation for the pattern of results reported by Smith et al. by including a manipulation of task emphasis: for half of the participants the prospective memory task was emphasize, while for the other half the ongoing color-matching task was emphasized. Older adults performed a 4-color version of the ongoing color-matching task, while young adults completed either the 4-color or a more difficult 6-color version of the ongoing task. Older adults failed to perform as well as the young adults on the prospective memory task regardless of task emphasis, even when older adults were performing as well or better than the young adults on the ongoing color-matching task. The current results indicate that the lack of an effect of ongoing task load on prospective memory task performance is not due to a perception that one or the other task is more important than the other. 相似文献
9.
JERKER RONNBERG 《Scandinavian journal of psychology》1980,21(1):83-95
In a series of five single-trial free recall experiments it was demonstrated how manipulations of predictability in a list learning task caused reliable effects on the recency portions of the nominal serial position curve. Predictability was defined as knowledge of when the next item in the list should appear ( interval predictability) and knowledge of when the list should end and thus when recall could start ( signal predictability). Predictability was manipulated by varying the inter-item intervals within a list according to different "encoding functions", e.g. a successive increase (Condition I ) or decrease (Condition D ) of the inter-item intervals. Predictability was also manipulated by means of post-list recall signals. The data obtained show, that depending on how the interval and the signal predictabilities were manipulated, either Condition I or Condition D lists could be made more beneficial to the recall of recency items than lists with constant inter-item intervals (Condition C ). Previous encoding, storage and retrieval models were found not to give satisfactory accounts of the obtained recency effects. Neither could any traditional rehearsal model explain the data. Instead it is proposed that the predictability of stimuli in a list learning task explains how changes in recall of recency items come about. It was also hypothesized that the temporal demands, afforded to the subjects by the different encoding functions, explained the obtained output order variations. 相似文献
10.
Kemper S Herman RE Nartowicz J 《Neuropsychology, development, and cognition. Section B, Aging, neuropsychology and cognition》2005,12(4):340-358
Young and older adults provided language samples in response to elicitation questions while concurrently performing 3 different tasks. The language samples were scored on three dimensions: fluency, grammatical complexity, and content. Previous research had suggested the hypothesis that the restricted speech register of older adults is buffered from the costs of dual task demands. This hypothesis was tested by comparing language samples collected during a baseline condition with those produced while the participants were performing the concurrent tasks. The results indicate that young and older adults adopt different strategies when confronted with dual task demands. Young adults shift to a restricted speech register when confronted with dual task demands. Older adults, who were already using a restricted speech register, became less fluent although the grammatical complexity and informational content of their speech was preserved. Hence, some but not all aspects of older adults' speech are buffered from dual task demands. 相似文献
11.
Children (7 to 10 years), young adults (17 to 24 years), and older adults (55 to 77 years) were asked to learn three lists of words that were of mixed modality (half the words were visual, and half the words were auditory). With one list the subjects were asked a semantic orienting question; with another, a nonsemantic orienting question; and with a third, no orienting question. Half the subjects in each age group were also asked to remember the presentation modality of each word. Older adults remembered less information about modality than children and young adults did, and the variation in the type of orienting question--or the lack of one--affected modality identification. However, there was no Orienting Task x Age interaction for modality identification. The results of this study suggest that encoding modality information does not take place automatically--in any age group--but that explanations focusing on encoding strategies and effort are not likely to account for older adults' difficulties in remembering presentation modality. 相似文献
12.
Sustained-attention capacity in young and older adults 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
The sustained-attention capacity of young (21-29 years) and older (65-78 years) adults was examined using a high-event rate digit-discrimination vigilance task presented at three levels of stimulus degradation. Increased stimulus degradation led to a reduction in the hit rate and to a greater decline in hit rate over blocks (increased vigilance decrement). Sensitivity (d') declined over blocks only at the highest level of stimulus degradation. Older adults had a lower hit rate and showed greater vigilance decrement than young adults, particularly when stimuli were highly degraded. However, both age groups showed the same pattern of stability in sensitivity when stimulus degradation was moderate, and sensitivity decrement over time when stimulus degradation was high. The results suggest that the process of sustained allocation of capacity--as reflected in temporal changes in sensitivity--operates similarly in young and older adults. 相似文献
13.
AntecedentsGiven the contradictions of previous studies on the changes in attentional responses produced in aging a Stroop emotional task was proposed to compare young and older adults to words or faces with an emotional valence.MethodThe words happy or sad were superimposed on faces that express the emotion of happiness or sadness. The emotion expressed by the word and the face could agree or not (cued and uncued trials, respectively). 85 young and 66 healthy older adults had to identify both faces and words separately, and the interference between the two types of stimuli was examined.ResultsAn interference effect was observed for both types of stimuli in both groups. There was more interference on positive faces and words than on negative stimuli.ConclusionsOlder adults had more difficulty than younger in focusing on positive uncued trials, whereas there was no difference across samples on negative uncued trials. 相似文献
14.
Kemper S Hoffman L Schmalzried R Herman R Kieweg D 《Neuropsychology, development, and cognition. Section B, Aging, neuropsychology and cognition》2011,18(3):257-279
A digital pursuit rotor was used to monitor speech planning and production costs by time-locking tracking performance to the auditory wave form produced as young and older adults were describing someone they admire. The speech sample and time-locked tracking record were segmented at utterance boundaries and multilevel modeling was used to determine how utterance-level predictors such as utterance duration or sentence grammatical complexity and person-level predictors such as speaker age or working memory capacity predicted tracking performance. Three models evaluated the costs of speech planning, the costs of speech production, and the costs of speech output monitoring. The results suggest that planning and producing propositionally dense utterances is more costly for older adults and that older adults experience increased costs as a result of having produced a long, informative, or rapid utterance. 相似文献
15.
In four experiments, semantic satiation was investigated in young and old adults. In the first two experiments, subjects were repeatedly presented a word (e.g.,dog) and then were presented a pair of words (e.g.,dog-cat ordog-chair) for a relatedness decision. The results of both experiments indicated that for the young adults, the relatedness effect (the difference between response latency on related and unrelated trials) decreased as a function of the number of times the satiated word was repeated, whereas for the older adults, there was no evidence of a decrease in the relatedness effect across repetitions of the satiated word. In the third experiment, we investigated whether phonological codes are also susceptible to satiation. This experiment was similar to the first two experiments with the exception that subjects made rhyme decisions (same-claim vs.same-dime) instead of semantic relatedness decisions. The results of this experiment did not yield any evidence of satiation for either the young adults or the older adults. The final experiment eliminated a simple decrease in attentional alertness or fatigue account of the semantic satiation effects found in the first two experiments. In this experiment, the repeated word was always unrelated to the pair of words presented for the relatedness decision. The results of this experiment did not yield any evidence of semantic satiation for either the young or the older adults. The discussion focuses on the mechanisms underlying semantic satiation and the implications of age-related changes in these mechanisms. 相似文献
16.
17.
The difficulty of reasoning tasks depends on their relational complexity, which increases with the number of relations that must be considered simultaneously to make an inference, and on the number of irrelevant items that must be inhibited. The authors examined the ability of younger and older adults to integrate multiple relations and inhibit irrelevant stimuli. Young adults performed well at all but the highest level of relational complexity, whereas older adults performed poorly even at a medium level of relational complexity, especially when irrelevant information was presented. Simulations based on a neurocomputational model of analogical reasoning, Learning and Inference with Schemas and Analogies (LISA), suggest that the observed decline in reasoning performance may be explained by a decline in attention and inhibitory functions in older adults. 相似文献
18.
Anat Berger-Mandelbaum 《Neuropsychology, development, and cognition. Section B, Aging, neuropsychology and cognition》2019,26(1):58-85
The present study explored self-initiated object-location memory in ecological contexts, as aspect of memory that is largely absent from the research literature. Young and older adults memorized objects-location associations they selected themselves or object-location associations provided to them, and elaborated on the strategy they used when selecting the locations themselves. Retrieval took place 30 min and 1 month after encoding. The results showed an age-related decline in self-initiated and provided object-location memory. Older adults benefited from self-initiation more than young adults when tested after 30 min, while the benefit was equal when tested after 1 month. Furthermore, elaboration enhanced memory only in older adults, and only after 30 min. Both age groups used deep encoding strategies on the majority of the trials, but their percentage was lower in older adults. Overall, the study demonstrated the processes involved in self-initiated object-location memory, which is an essential part of everyday functioning. 相似文献
19.
Emerging evidence highlights that arm movements exert a substantial and functionally relevant contribution on quiet standing balance control in young adults. Ageing is associated with “non-functional” compensatory postural control strategies (i.e., lower limb co-contraction), which in turn, may increase the reliance on an upper body strategy to control upright stance. Thus, the primary purpose of this study was to compare the effects of free versus restricted arm movements on balance performance in young and older adults, during tasks of different difficulty. Fifteen young (mean ± SD age; 21.3 ± 4.2 years) and fifteen older (mean ± SD age; 73.3 ± 5.0 years) adults performed bipedal, semi-tandem and tandem balance tasks under two arm position conditions: restricted arm movements and free arm movements. Centre of pressure (COP) amplitude and frequency were calculated as indices of postural performance and strategy, respectively. Especially in older adults, restriction of arm movement resulted in increased sway amplitude and frequency, which was primarily observed for the mediolateral direction. Further, increasing balance task difficulty raised the arm restriction cost (ARC; a new measure to quantify free vs. restricted arm movement differences in postural control) that was more prominent in older adults. These findings indicate the ARC provides a measure of reliance on the upper body for balance control and that arm movement is important for postural control in older adults, especially during tasks of greater difficulty. 相似文献
20.
Carla M. Strickland-Hughes Robin Lea West Kimberly A. Smith Natalie C. Ebner 《Memory (Hove, England)》2017,25(8):1072-1088
Feedback is an important self-regulatory process that affects task effort and subsequent performance. Benefits of positive feedback for list recall have been explored in research on goals and feedback, but the effect of negative feedback on memory has rarely been studied. The current research extends knowledge of memory and feedback effects by investigating face–name association memory and by examining the potential mediation of feedback effects, in younger and older adults, through self-evaluative beliefs. Beliefs were assessed before and after name recognition and name recall testing. Repeated presentation of false positive feedback was compared to false negative feedback and a no feedback condition. Results showed that memory self-efficacy declined over time for participants in the negative and no feedback conditions but was sustained for those receiving positive feedback. Furthermore, participants who received negative feedback felt older after testing than before testing. For name recall, the positive feedback group outperformed the negative feedback and no feedback groups combined, with no age interactions. The observed feedback-related effects on memory were fully mediated by changes in memory self-efficacy. These findings advance our understanding of how beliefs are related to feedback in memory and inform future studies examining the importance of self-regulation in memory. 相似文献