首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 171 毫秒
1.
关于两种Angoff法比较的模拟实验研究   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
采用模拟实验法比较研究了两种Angoff法——概率法和对错法——设定分数线的准确性和稳定性,结果表明:(1)当真能力低于测验的平均难度时,概率法高估分数线,而对错法低估分数线;反之,当真能力高于测验平均难度时,概率法低估,而对错法高估;(2)当真能力接近测验平均难度时,概率法比对错法更准确;反之,当真能力远高于或低于测验平均难度时,对错法更准确;(3)无论在何种实验条件下,概率法均比对错法更稳定。  相似文献   

2.
本文提出了"主观词"的概念,并探讨主观词存在的心理现实性以及主观词的加工特征。包括3项研究:研究1考察了普通大学生对双字结构的词汇辨别情况,发现他们倾向于将短语和词主观判断为词,即他们按照个体对词的独特表征认知词,证实了主观词存在的心理现实性。研究2和研究3分别采用"词优效应"研究中使用的强迫选择作业和词汇判断任务考察主观词加工的特征。研究2发现,读者加工词和主观词(语法上是短语但主观评定为词)的正确率和反应时没有差异,但与非词条件相比,表现为正确率高,反应时短;研究3的反应时结果同研究2,而3种条件的正确率均在90%以上且不存在显著差异。在本研究条件下,得出以下结论:(1)主观词的存在具有心理现实性:读者对汉语词语的表征是主观的,不一定符合词的语法学规定;(2)在强迫选择作业下,与非词相比,表现出词优效应,主观词作为整体被识别。(3)在词汇判断任务下,主观词与词相同,与非词相比,表现出加工优势。  相似文献   

3.
主观幸福感的干预研究述评   总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3  
王彦  王岩 《心理科学》2008,31(6):1441-1442,1440
适应和设定点理论质疑主观幸福感能否改变,但研究证明一些干预措施可以增强主观幸福感.持续幸福模型总结了影响幸福感的三类因素,建议利用意向性活动改变幸福感.另有其它可行的干预方法.已有研究在方法和理论等方面存在局限,且缺少跨文化比较.  相似文献   

4.
主观概率判断的支持理论   总被引:2,自引:1,他引:1  
支持理论是一个关于主观概率判断的非外延性理论,它有以下几个主要观点:1.主观概率判断受到描述的影响,具有描述的依赖性;2.主观概率判断的结果是判断者对中心假设的相对支持的反映;3.主观概率在二元判断中表现出二元互补性,在多元判断中表现出次可加性;4.主观概率判断存在分解效应和促进效应  相似文献   

5.
随机支持模型试图以假设支持为随机变量的方式描述主观概率的校准。采用随机配对的60个中国城市户籍人口的比较任务验证了该模型对初中、高中和大学生的适用性。研究还表明青少年主观概率校准的发展特点:青少年的正确率随年龄的增长而提高;主观概率判断(信心)不同年龄有显著差异,大学生信心最强,其次是初中生、再次是高中生;大学生主观概率判断的极端性最强,其次是初中生,再次是高中生;青少年的主观概率校准是过度自信的。  相似文献   

6.
目的探讨民族高校大学生睡眠质量与主观幸福感的关系。方法问卷调查法。结果民族地区大学生PSQI总分为(5.69±2.90),主观幸福感总分为(4.70±O.83);PSQI总分与主观幸福感得分呈显著负相关;PSQI总分和入睡时间依次进入主观幸福感的回归方程,对主观幸福感总分具有显著的预测作用。结论睡眠质量与主观幸福感显著相关,睡眠质量是主观幸福感有效的预测指标之一。  相似文献   

7.
项目的激活强度与元记忆判断   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
本实验采用不同于国外由Hart首创的FOK(Feelling-of-Knowing)研究方法的新方法──模糊再认判断法(Fuzzy-Recognitionjudgement,简称FR)来研究无记忆判断及其产生机制.实验中安排了范畴判断与字词识记相结合,造成四种不同激活强度的字词,让被试对其进行FR判断,并用迫选再认作为标准测验进行验证.结果表明,范畴判断的语义激活增强了字词的激活强度,从而影响FR判断的等级.识记项目的激活强度愈高,相应的FR判断等级也愈高.  相似文献   

8.
我国主观幸福感的研究现状与趋势   总被引:13,自引:0,他引:13  
幸福的探讨是哲学、社会学、心理学等多学科关注的热点,而对于主观幸福感的研究在积极心理学领域中占有重要位置。本文首先阐述了主观幸福感概念的形成与发展;然后重点介绍了近几年我国在主观幸福感研究方面的一些现状与进展,主要涉及国内有关主观幸福感影响因素的分析以及较常用测量工具的介绍;最后从研究内容的拓展、研究方法的多样化、研究工具的本土化和研究进程的加快等四个方面,展望了我国有关主观幸福感未来的研究趋势。  相似文献   

9.
采用问卷调查法,以692名中小学教师作为被试,探讨教师职业人格和主观幸福感的关系及其机制。结果发现:(1)专家型教师在职业人格得分上显著高于新手、熟手型教师,体现出与教师职业更匹配、更具适应性的人格特点;(2)职业人格与主观幸福感正相关显著,职业人格可通过影响工作投入程度进而影响主观幸福感,工作投入在二者之间起部分中介作用;(3)新手、熟手和专家型教师职业人格影响主观幸福感的作用机制存在差异:新手职业人格直接影响主观幸福感,熟手大部分通过工作投入影响主观幸福感,而专家完全通过工作投入影响主观幸福感。研究表明,教师职业人格可通过提高工作投入程度,进而提升其主观幸福感。  相似文献   

10.
本研究基于Jones(1991)的问题权变模型,采用情景研究法,考察主观道德强度的结构以及主观道德强度对企业道德决策的预测作用.研究选取137名MBA学生为被试,要求被试基于所提供情景中假想参与者的行为,回答随后关于道德决策(道德识别、道德判断和道德意图阶段)的问题.结果表明:1)主观道德强度是1维结构;2)主观道德强度对道德识别、道德判断和道德意图有显著预测作用 ;3)结果大小对道德识别、道德判断和道德意图有显著预测作用,而社会舆论对道德识别和道德意图有显著预测作用.  相似文献   

11.
Several previous investigations have measured accelerating psychophysical functions for perceived force with exponents of about 1.7. Two halving and doubling experiments presented here imply a psychophysical function for perceived force with an exponent between 0.6 and 0.8. That is, more than a doubling of force was needed to double the sensation, and similarly for halving. In the first experiment, subjects squeezed rigid instrumented cylinders between the thumb and first two fingers of each hand. They generated and released a reference force with one hand, and then squeezed the opposite hand to produce a sensation magnitude equal to, twice that, or half that of the reference. An analysis using a model that accounted for compression bias yielded average psychophysical functions with exponents of 0.58 and 0.59 (nondominant and dominant hands, respectively). The second experiment was an attempt to replicate earlier results and to reconcile them with the first experiment by using a paradigm duplicated from a previous study. Subjects in the second experiment made unilateral halving and doubling judgments of handgrip while squeezing a hand dynamometer. Again, the halving and doubling judgments yielded decelerating functions with exponents of 0.75 and 0.80 (nondominant and dominant hands, respectively). Even though the results of the first two experiments contradict earlier investigations, they can be explained by an equilibrium model of motor control assuming that subjects halve and double the central motor command rather than the sensation of force. The force is simply the result of the mechanical equilibrium established between the load and the compliant effector (the hand). The predicted relationship between the motor command judgments, the compliance of the hand, and the resultant forces was confirmed in a third experiment in which the mechanical compliance of the three-finger pinch was measured by using a pneumatic manipulandum to apply force perturbations in a “do-notintervene” paradigm. The measured compliance characteristic was accelerating, just as predicted by the model, in order to produce a decelerating psychophysical function for “perceived force.” In this experiment, then, judgments of perceived force appear to be judgments of the central motor command.  相似文献   

12.
Differences in spatial knowledge acquired from maps and navigation   总被引:7,自引:0,他引:7  
Models of the spatial knowledge people acquire from maps and navigation and the procedures required for spatial judgments using this knowledge are proposed. From a map, people acquire survey knowledge encoding global spatial relations. This knowledge resides in memory in images that can be scanned and measured like a physical map. From navigation, people acquire procedural knowledge of the routes connecting diverse locations. People combine mental simulation of travel through the environment and informal algebra to compute spatial judgments. An experiment in which subjects learned an environment from navigation or from a map evaluates predictions of these models. With moderate exposure, map learning is superior for judgments of relative location and straight-line distances among objects. Learning from navigation is superior for orienting oneself with respect to unseen objects and estimating route distances. With extensive exposure, the performance superiority of maps over navigation vanishes. These and other results are consonant with the proposed mechanisms.  相似文献   

13.
On the Affine Structure of Perceptual Space   总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3  
Affine geometry is a generalization of Euclidean geometry in which distance can be scaled along parallel directions, though relative distances in different directions may be incommensurable. This article presents a new procedure for testing the intrinsic affine structure of a psychological space by having subjects perform bisection judgments over multiple directions. If those judgments are internally consistent with one another, they must satisfy a theorem first proved by Pierre Varignon around 300 years ago. In the experiment reported here, this procedure was employed to measure the perceived structure of a visual ground surface. The results revealed that observers' judgments were systematically distorted relative to the physical environment, but that the judged bisections in different directions had an internally consistent affine structure. Implications of these findings for other possible response tasks are considered.  相似文献   

14.
This study investigated the extent to which supervisors use feedback from outside sources in making performance judgments. A simulation was conducted in which participants with organizational supervisory experience made an initial performance judgment about a profiled employee. Participants then received additional information that was discrepant from their initial judgment (positive or negative) from one of two sources (the profiled employee himself or one of his peers). The direction of the discrepant information and its source interacted in determining final ratings, such that, participants were more likely to use discrepant information to alter their performance judgments in a consistent direction when the source was a peer than when the source was the employee himself. Furthermore, participants' opinions about the usefulness of peer information for performance judgments moderated this interaction. Specifically, participants who believed that information from an employee's peers was useful, were more likely to use discrepant information provided by a peer when making final performance judgments than were participants who did not believe that information from an employee's peers was useful.  相似文献   

15.
Gamache PL  Grondin S 《Perception》2010,39(11):1431-1451
To further explore how memory influences time judgments, we conducted two experiments on the lifespan of temporal representations in memory. Penney et al (2000, Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance 26 1770-1787) reported that the perceived duration of auditorily and visually marked intervals differs only when both marker-type intervals are compared directly. This finding can be explained by a 'memory-mixing' process, whereby the memory trace of previous intervals influences the perception of upcoming ones, which are then added to the memory content. In the experiments discussed here, we manipulated the mixing mode of auditory/visual signal presentations. In experiment 1, signals from the same modality were either grouped by blocks or randomised within blocks. The results showed that the auditory/visual difference decreased but remained present when modalities were grouped by blocks. In experiment 2, we used a line-segmentation task. The results showed that, after a training block was performed in one modality, the perceived duration of signals from the other modality was distorted for at least 30 trials and that the magnitude of the difference decreased as the block went on. The results of both experiments highlight the influence of memory on time judgments, providing empirical support to, and quantitative portrayal of, the memory-mixing process.  相似文献   

16.
Participants saw a standard scene of three objects on a desktop and then judged whether a comparison scene was either the same, except for the viewpoint of the scene, or different, when one or more of the objects either exchanged places or were rotated around their center. As in Nakatani, Pollatsek, and Johnson (2002), judgment times were longer when the rotation angles of the comparison scene increased, and the size of the rotation effect varied for different axes and was larger for same judgments than for different judgments. A second experiment, which included trials without the desktop, indicated that removing the desktop frame of reference mainly affected the y-axis rotation conditions (the axis going vertically through the desktop plane). In addition, eye movement analyses indicated that the process was far more than a simple analogue rotation of the standard scene. The total response latency was divided into three components: the initial eye movement latency, the first-pass time, and the second-pass time. The only indication of a rotation effect in the time to execute the first two components was for z-axis (plane of sight) rotations. Thus, for x- and y-axis rotations, rotation effects occurred only in the probability of there being a second pass and the time to execute it. The data are inconsistent either with an initial rotation of the memory representation of the standard scene to the orientation of the comparison scene or with a holistic alignment of the comparison scene prior to comparing it with the memory representation of the standard scene. Indeed, the eye movement analysis suggests that little of the increased response time for rotated comparison scenes is due to something like a time-consuming analogue process but is, instead, due to more comparisons on individual objects being made (possibly more double checking).  相似文献   

17.
Three studies examined intuitive judgments of rate change. These studies considered how people intuitively decide that the occurrence rate of particular events is on the increase or in decline. Using the topic of teenage pregnancy, participants were given information about the population size and the frequency of pregnancies at a hypothetical high school in three studies at two points in time. In the first of these studies details about population size were made highly salient, and here population information was found to directly influence intuitive judgments of rate change. In the second study population information was presented in a more realistic fashion. Under these conditions judgments that the pregnancy rate had changed were found to be based on changes in the number of pregnancies. Whenever the number of pregnancies changed, regardless of compensating changes in school size, people inferred a change in rate. In the third of these studies, population details were not provided; only information about number of pregnancies was presented. Subjects' queries for additional information were then assessed. Despite the absence of population information, few subjects requested population details when asked what additional information they would like in order to judge the relative seriousness of the pregnancy problem. Taken together, the three studies suggest that when making intuitive judgments of change in rate, people may be relatively insensitive to the importance of population information. The implications of such findings are discussed.  相似文献   

18.
In order to prevent subjects from actually performing a judgment of vernier alignment between the double images rather than matching stereoscopic depth of the test objects, various precautions have previously been taken, e.g., using inexperienced subjects who were ignorant of the experimental situation and possible matching criteria. In the experiment reported here, a subject was given training in observing double images and the matching problems were also discussed. The subject was then instructed to perform stereoscopic depth judgments. The comparison between these data and data based on vernier alignment judgments under similar conditions showed that knowledge about the experimental situation and the vernier alignment criterion did not interfere with the subject’s task of matching the stereoscopic depth between visible double images.  相似文献   

19.
《Learning and motivation》1987,18(2):147-166
Two experiments investigated the way in which judgments of the extent to which an action caused an outcome changed as more experience of the actionoutcome contingency was presented. In the first experiment judgments increased across trials when there was a positive contingency and decreased when there was a negative contingency. In noncontingent situations judgments were biased by the overall probability of the outcome. In the second experiment the changes across trials under positive and negative contingencies persisted even when the subjects were given the opportunity to dissociate their causality judgments from their degree of confidence in those judgments. The results are at variance with the dP and dD theories which attempt to account for causality judgments in terms of statistical rules based on the probabilities or frequencies of the relevant events. If such theories were modified, however, to take account of the regression of the subjects' estimates onto the actual probabilities or frequencies, then the data could be accommodated. On the other hand, a simple associative view is also able to account for the data.  相似文献   

20.
Maximum likelihood estimation in multidimensional scaling   总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4  
A variety of distributional assumptions for dissimilarity judgments are considered, with the lognormal distribution being favored for most situations. An implicit equation is discussed for the maximum likelihood estimation of the configuration with or without individual weighting of dimensions. A technique for solving this equation is described and a number of examples offered to indicate its performance in practice. The estimation of a power transformation of dissimilarity is also considered. A number of likelihood ratio hypothesis tests are discussed and a small Monte Carlo experiment described to illustrate the behavior of the test of dimensionality in small samples.The research reported here was supported by grant number APA 320 to the author by the National Research Council of Canada.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号