首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   46799篇
  免费   1021篇
  国内免费   9篇
  2019年   288篇
  2018年   3789篇
  2017年   3142篇
  2016年   2625篇
  2015年   500篇
  2014年   487篇
  2013年   2126篇
  2012年   1243篇
  2011年   3099篇
  2010年   2925篇
  2009年   1919篇
  2008年   2388篇
  2007年   2780篇
  2006年   679篇
  2005年   745篇
  2004年   707篇
  2003年   630篇
  2002年   619篇
  2001年   708篇
  2000年   722篇
  1999年   546篇
  1998年   301篇
  1997年   329篇
  1996年   304篇
  1995年   274篇
  1992年   517篇
  1991年   462篇
  1990年   464篇
  1989年   434篇
  1988年   448篇
  1987年   415篇
  1986年   460篇
  1985年   438篇
  1984年   382篇
  1983年   344篇
  1981年   277篇
  1979年   407篇
  1978年   329篇
  1977年   292篇
  1976年   303篇
  1975年   352篇
  1974年   440篇
  1973年   473篇
  1972年   350篇
  1971年   369篇
  1970年   336篇
  1969年   364篇
  1968年   435篇
  1967年   400篇
  1966年   415篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 125 毫秒
41.
42.
Four experiments were conducted to determine whether echoic memory plays a role in differences between good and poor readers. The first two experiments used a suffix procedure in which the subject is read a list of digits with either a tone control or the word go appended to the list. For lists that exceeded the length of the subjects memory span by one digit (i.e., that avoided ceiling effects), the poor readers showed a larger decrement in the suffix condition than did the good readers. The third experiment was directed at the question of whether the duration of echoic memory is different for good and poor readers. Children shadowed words presented to one ear at a rate determined to give 75-85% shadowing accuracy. The items presented to the nonattended ear were words and an occasional digit. At various intervals after the presentation of the digit, a light signaled that the subject was to cease shadowing and attempt to recall any digit that had occurred in the nonattended ear recently. Whereas good and poor readers recalled the digit equally if tested immediately after presentation, the poor readers showed a faster decline in recall of the digit as retention interval increased. A fourth experiment was conducted to determine whether the differences in echoic memory were specific to speech stimuli or occurred at a more basic level of aural persistence. Bursts of white noise were separated by 9-400 ms of silence and the subject was to say whether there were one or two sounds presented. There were no differences in detectability functions for good and poor readers.  相似文献   
43.
We present an experiment showing that need for closure (NFC)—defined as the epistemic desire for certainty—can moderate individuals' affective reactions to cognitive inconsistency. Informed by Kruglanski and colleagues' new theory, that cognitive inconsistency elicits negative affect particularly under certain circumstances, we find that NFC (i.e. the desire for certain, stable and unambiguous knowledge) influences the strength of consistency effects and resulting negative affect. More specifically, we find that individuals who are high on NFC experience more negative affect upon encountering an inconsistent (vs. consistent) cognition. However, when individuals are low on NFC, inconsistency is irrelevant, and their affect depends on whether the ultimate outcome of the cognition is positive or negative. We discuss the theoretical and practical implications of this research.  相似文献   
44.
45.
46.
47.
48.
49.
50.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号